Available online at www.s m u AU niv an development In this study, we videotaped social play within litters of play (Burghardt 2005). Animals may play to learn valu- ARTICLE IN PRESS ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 20 doi:10.1016/j.anbehaStreet, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1043, U.S.A.unexpected situations (Spinka et al. 2001) or as a way to assess their own capabilities relative to conspeci?cs (Thompson 1998; Smith et al. 1999; Palagi et al. 2004). Although all of these hypotheses seem reasonable, it is extremely dif?cult to determine the reproductive bene?ts of social play. In a longitudinal study of social play in Correspondence: C. Ward, Department of Psychology, 530 Church (email: rameses@umich.edu). E. B. Bauer is at the Department of Ani- mal Programs, Smithsonian National Zoological Park, NZP-Lion/Tiger MRC: 5507, P.O. Box 37012, Washington, D.C. 20013-7012, U.S.A.domestic dogs to examine its role in the development of social relationships. We studied four litters of different breeds (one was a mixed-breed litter), and we continued observations on one litter for longer (40 weeks) than most previous studies have done. To our knowledge, this able social skills (Biben 1998) or to strengthen (Bekoff 1984) or test social bonds (Zahavi 1977; Pozis-Francois et al. 2004), as training for cognitive (Bekoff 1984; Spinka et al. 2001) or motor development (Byers 1998), to de- velop the emotional ?exibility needed for dealing withSocial play is play directed at a conspeci?c (Bekoff & Byers 1981; Fagen 1981; Bekoff 1984) and in canids includes behaviours such as chasing and play-?ghting games, mounting behaviour (i.e. mimicking copulatory behav- iour) and inhibited biting (Bekoff 1974; Burghardt 2005). is the ?rst study in domestic dogs to compare systemati- cally numerous aspects of social play (such as play-partner preferences, role reversals and self-handicapping) both across time and across different litters. Hypotheses abound concerning the functions of socialrelationships within litters. We collected data when the puppies were between 3 and 40 weeks of age, but collection times varied by litter. We divided data analysis into three time periods to coincide approx- imately with critical periods in the early social development of dogs. Early play-partner preferences were associated with preferences in later time periods, and the tendency for puppies to prefer speci?c partners increased over time. Play did not conform to 50e50 symmetry of roles between partners, which some researchers claim is necessary to sustain play. In the later juvenile period (time 3), dogs who engaged in high rates of offense behaviours (e.g. chasing, forcing partners down) also initiated play at higher rates, implying that winning during play may become more important as puppies mature. Self-handicapping behaviours were positively associated with play signalling, suggesting that, like play signals, self-handicap- ping may function to indicate playful intent. In mixed-sex dyads, males initiated play, engaged in offense behaviours, and self-handicapped more than females. Females were more likely to initiate with females across all time periods, but males were more likely to initiate with males only in time 3. We discuss results from mixed- and same-sex interactions with reference to inter- and intrasexual competition. The types of offense and self-handicapping behaviours displayed were similar across litters, suggesting that the expres- sion of these behaviours may follow a similar ontogeny in puppies in general.  2008 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: asymmetrical play; Canis lupus familiaris; domestic dog; partner preferences; self-handicapping; socialWe videotaped behaviour in four litters of domesti0003e3 Please c Anim. Bc dogs to explore social play and the development of(Received 30 November 2007; initial acceptance 27 February 2008; ?nal acceptance 13 May 2008; published online - - -; MS. number: A07-00006)Partner preferences and asym domestic dog, Canis lup CAMILLE WARD* , ERIKA B. B *Department of Psychology, U yDepartment of Animal Programs, Smithsoni1 472/08/$34.00/0  2008 The Association for the Stu ite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym ehav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004ciencedirect.com etries in social play among s familiaris, littermates ER? & BARBARA B. SMUTS* ersity of Michigan, Ann Arbor National Zoological Park, Washington, D.C. 08, --, --e-- v.2008.06.004dy of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, ARTICLE IN PRESS 2multiple litters of wild meerkats (also social carnivores), Sharpe (2005a, b, c) concluded that several of the most frequently proposed functions of social play were not supported by the data, but she did not propose any alter- native explanations for why young meerkats play. Only one study has linked play directly to survival (brown bear cubs, Ursus arctos, that play more in their ?rst sum- mer are more likely to survive until the end of the next summer), but the mechanisms involved remain un- known (Fagen & Fagen 2004). Evidence for some species indicates that mammals play more and initiate play more often with individuals they can dominate during play (Owens 1975; Biben 1986, 1998). Even so, experiencing subordinate positions during play may also confer long-term bene?ts because individ- uals gain the opportunity to practice defensive strategies that could later be used during an actual ?ght. Also, indi- viduals who consistently maintain a dominant position during play may ?nd themselves at a loss for willing play partners (Biben 1998). Some researchers contend that in order for play to occur, both participants must win an equal proportion (50%) of play encounters (Bekoff 2001). This is called the 50e50 rule (Aldis 1975; Pellis & Pellis 1998). Individuals can balance the time spent in dominant and subordinate roles during play by self- handicapping and reversing roles. Self-handicapping occurs when individuals place themselves in disadvan- taged positions or situations that could make them more vulnerable to attack by ?opponents? (Fagen 1981; Bekoff & Allen 1998; Spinka et al. 2001; Bauer & Smuts 2007). For example, red-necked wallabies, Macropus rufogriseus banksianus, self-handicap when playing with younger partners by standing in a defensive, ?at-footed posture and pawing rather than sparring (Watson & Croft 1996), thereby allowing younger partners to gain a competitive advantage. Role reversals occur when animals change dominant and subordinate positions during play ?ghting (Balfour 1987; Biben 1998; Burghardt 2005) or when ani- mals dominant outside of play assume subordinate roles during play (Bauer & Smuts 2007). In some cases, the op- portunity to play may be more important than winning. For example, among squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus (Biben 1998), and hamadryas baboons, Papio hamadryas hamadryas (Pereira & Preisser 1998), stronger individuals ?soften? their play style as a mechanism to promote play relations when choice of play partners is restricted. Of the studies mentioned above, only Bauer & Smuts (2007) published quantitative data on adherence to the 50e50 rule, which is why additional data (like those pro- vided here) are important. Bauer & Smuts (2007) found that adult dogs do not play in accordance with the pre- dictions of the 50e50 rule, although the degree of diver- gence from egalitarian play varies among dyads (only 5.4% of the 55 dyads tested displayed 50e50 symmetry during play, whereas 21.8% displayed complete asymme- try). The 50e50 rule predicts that the dominant member of a dyad will self-handicap more (to bring the probabil- ity of winning for the subordinate closer to 50%), but they found the reverse effect. The 50e50 rule also pre- ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, --, -dicts that dominance relationships outside of play will be absent (or at least very relaxed) during play, but they Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004found that dominance relationships were still evident during play. Although it is not yet known why dog dyads display such wide variation in degrees of asymmetry dur- ing play, the study by Bauer & Smuts (2007) makes it clear that ?fair play? (Bekoff 2001) is not necessary for play to occur. Because social play often includes motor patterns used in predatory, mating and agonistic contexts, play signals have evolved to help animals convey playful intentions (West 1974; Bekoff & Allen 1998). For example, Bekoff (1995) found that among captive coyotes, Canis latrans, wolves, Canis lupus, and domestic dogs, the play bow sig- nal was more likely to occur immediately preceding or fol- lowing behaviours that could most easily be misinterpreted as real aggression, such as when an animal shook the head while biting another. In adult domestic dogs, the individual within a dyad who showed more self-handicapping behaviour also play-signalled more of- ten (Bauer & Smuts 2007). For a given species, sex differences in play should be more prominent when adult females and males differ in physical characteristics (e.g. body size), behavioural char- acteristics (e.g. involvement in hunting activity, intraspe- ci?c ?ghting, territorial defence) or social preferences (e.g. formation of strong bonds with same- versus opposite-sex individuals; Maestripieri & Ross 2004; Burghardt 2005). Among canids, males and females show little size dimor- phism and engage in similar roles (Derix et al. 1993; McLeod & Fentress 1997), and some research has sug- gested no sex differences in play style (Bekoff 1974; Biben 1983; Bauer & Smuts 2007). However, among infant domestic dogs, sex differences in play have been reported (Lund & Vestergaard 1998; Pal 2008). In domestic dogs, social and sexual play (e.g. mounting) ?rst emerge during the socialization period that begins at 3 weeks and ends at approximately 12 weeks of age (Freedman et al. 1961; Scott & Fuller 1965). During this time, puppies learn social skills and form bonds with other dogs (Lindsay 2000), and play-partner preferences may begin to form. Following the socialization period, the juvenile period lasts from approximately 12 weeks to 6 months or later (until sexual maturity; Scott & Marston 1950; Scott & Fuller 1965). During this time, play con- tinues to be common. Some research suggests that in domestic and wild canids, play contributes to the formation of dominance relationships within litters (domestic dogs: Scott & Fuller 1965; Bekoff 1972; wild red foxes: Meyer & Weber 1996). However, no one, to our knowledge, has systemat- ically quanti?ed the development of dominance relations among littermates in wolves or domestic dogs during nat- urally occurring social interactions. In our study, virtually every intraspeci?c social interaction puppies had (other than resting in body contact or snif?ng) occurred in the context of play. Therefore, we made no attempt to analyse dominance relationships per se, although we did examine role asymmetries during play (see below). We examined the development of social relationships during play among littermates in three ways. First, we examined play-partner preferences in puppies, diversity in choice of partners and the stability of preferences over time. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, Second, we examined asymmetries in social play. Speci?- cally, we determinedwhether play conformed to the 50e50 rule; we examined the relationship between various play behaviours, including play initiations, ?offense behaviours? (similar to ?attacks and pursuits? in Bauer & Smuts 2007), self-handicapping and play bows; and we determined whether play behaviours varied by sex. Last, we examined how the individual actions that comprised offense and self-handicapping behaviours varied by litter and time. METHODS Subjects riods riods associated with the development of social behaviours collected in the adjacent yard, a 506-m2 fenced area. For all litters, living areas were large enough to allow puppies to move and play freely. During time period 2 (Table 1), owners of puppies from litters 1 and 2 brought them back to the breeders? homes once or twice a month and placed them together for a cou- ple of hours in the outdoor enclosures described above (extended observations of litters 3 and 4 were not possi- ble). This allowed us to collect longitudinal data on litter- mate play behaviour starting at 10e11 weeks through 23 weeks of age. For litter 1, all six littermates attended four sessions. For litter 2, ?ve puppies attended three sessions, and all six puppies came to the remaining session. Finally, for litter 1, we continued once-a-month obser- vations from 27 to 40 weeks of age (time period 3, Table 1) tify- Litter Breed Birth date* Sex compositiony Ob F M M M ARTICLE IN PRESS WARD & SMUTS: PLAY AMONG DOMESTIC DOG SIBLINGS 31 Shepherd mix 14 Feb 2004 3 \ 3 _ 2 Labrador retriever 21 Apr 2005 2 \ 4 _ 3 Doberman pincher 22 Apr 2004 1 \ 2 _ 4 Malamute 4 May 2005 4 _Table 1. Summary of domestic dog litters(Scott & Fuller 1965) and also to equate data collected across litters. Time 1 included the socialization period, time 2, the late socialization and early juvenile period, and time 3, the later juvenile period. For all litters, we observed puppies in the breeders? homes from 3 to 7e8 weeks of age (time period 1; see Table 1). During time 1, litters 1e3 were housed indoors in one-room enclosures of approximately 7.2 m2. Litters 2 and 3 also had free access to outdoor fenced areas 99 and 72 and 99 m2 in size, respectively. Litter 4 was ken- nelled outdoors in a 4-m2 enclosure, but data wereWe divided the data collection into three time pe (Table 1) to coincide approximately with critical peWe observed four litters of domestic dogs (three pure- bred litters and one mixed-breed litter; Table 1). All dams lived in ordinary households, and all puppies whelped naturally (no caesarean births) at home. The dams suckled the puppies and weaned them prior to placement in per- manent homes. Breeders supplemented nursing with solid foods starting around 4 weeks of age. Puppies from litters 2e4 remained sexually intact for the duration of the study. With the exception of one male, all of the puppies from litter 1 were spayed or neutered, starting at 21 weeks of age. Data Collection*Only surviving puppies are listed. yOne puppy (female) from litter 1 died shortly after birth, and two pupp zObservations were collected over three time periods. Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004Behavioural Coding We coded data from videotapes into a Microsoft Excel (version 2003) spreadsheet. We coded data only for play bouts that involved mutual, social play lasting for at least 2 s. Based on the log survivorship analysis (Martin & Bateson 1993), we considered a subsequent play bout between a focal and the same play partner to be indepen- dent if the interval separating successive bouts was at least 1 min. To investigate play initiations and partner preferences, we coded play-partner identities, the puppy who initiated play, how play was initiated (see Bekoff 1972) and the servation dates Time periodz Age range of puppies (weeks) ebeNov 2004 1 3e8 2 11e23 3 27e40 ayeOct 2005 1 3e8 2 10e23 ayeJun 2004 1 3e7 ayeJun 2005 1 3e83, were marked by the breeder with nail polish in iden ing locations on their bodies.under the conditions described above. All littermates attended two sessions and ?ve attended one session. For litters 1e3 (hereafter referred to as ?focal litters?) for all time periods that applied, we conducted 5-min focal samples on each puppy randomly selected during a given session. We videotaped focal animals and those who interacted with them using Canon ZR50 and Canon ZR95 digital video cameras. We had limited access to litter 4 and therefore decided to maximize data by videotaping play on an ad libitum basis (Altmann 1974). For identi?ca- tion, puppies wore coloured collars or, in the case of litteries (both females) from litter 2 died shortly after birth. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, times that play began and ended with each partner. To ant fo g s ?s b he use rou aw ith sad k m m a and lay ear ctl ARTICLE IN PRESS 4investigate behaviours that involved asymmetric roles (hereafter ?asymmetries?), we divided them into (1) offense behaviours and (2) self-handicapping behaviours (cf. Bauer & Smuts 2007). For these and other behaviours (e.g. play bows) we used a detailed ethogram (Table 2) based on prior research on domestic dogs (Bekoff 1972; Abrantes 1997; Bauer & Smuts 2007) and wolves (Schenkel 1967; van Hooff & Wensing 1987).Table 2. Ethogram of asymmetric behaviours in play Behaviour Offense behaviours Behaviours used to maintain a domin Bite shake D* bites Sy and shakes head back and Chase D runs after S with a least two runnin Chin over D places the underside of chin over S S?s shoulders, but sometimes over S?s Forced down D uses physical force or contact to ca a moving, standing or sitting position Mount D rears up (keeping hindlegs on the g spine with curved front legs and forep Muzzle bite D places mouth around S?s muzzle Over D sits on, stands over, or lies over S w Self-handicapping Behaviours that place an actor in a di Muzzle lick S licks on or around D?s muzzle. A lic Voluntary down S drops completely to the ground fro without D?s physical enforcement. D Play signal Used to begin play or to maintain a p Play bow Dog is crouched down, touching or n end high in the air. Orientation is dire *D: dog in dominant or winning position. yS: dog in subordinate or losing position. ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, --, -Data Analysis Play-partner preferences For focal litters, we calculated a play-partner preference (PPP) score for each animal in each litter using the following index (cf. Thompson 1996), Iij ? Bij Bi=?k 1? where k is the total number of puppies in a litter, Bij is the number of play bouts initiated by the ith puppy with the jth puppy as the recipient and Bi is the total number of play bouts initiated by the ith puppy. A puppy who initi- ated with all other partners an equal number of times would have all Iij score equal to 1.0. Scores were greater than 1.0 in situations in which individuals initiated play with some individuals more often than with others. Fol- lowing Thompson (1996), we used PPP scores of 2.0 to de?ne ?strong partner preferences?. We did not calculate PPP scores for litter 2, time 2 (because on most occasions, only ?ve of the six puppies were present). We used rowwise matrix correlation tests (Kr; MatMan software package with 10 000 permutations; Hemelrijk 1990a, b; de Vries 1993) to examine several aspects of play-partner preferences. Matrices were constructed sepa- rately by litter and time period for litters 1 and 2 (although Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004we calculated PPP scores for litter 3, sample size was too small, N ? 3, to conduct matrix analyses (Hemelrijk 1990a), and, as mentioned above, we did not collect focal data on litter 4). To determine if PPP scores were reciprocal (i.e. if puppy A preferred puppy B as a play partner, did B also tend to prefer A?), we compared a data matrix of PPP scores (with actors in rows and recipients in columns) with a second matrix that was a transposition of the ?rst. To test whether PPP scores were correlated across time periods for litter 1, we created a PPP matrix (as described above) for each time Definition or superior position over a partner rth while maintaining a hold on S trides while S runs or trots away from D ack, usually right behind the neck or near ad S to drop completely to the ground from nd) to place forelegs on S?s back. D has a rounded s to grasp S?s torso. Pelvic thrusting may or may not be present at least 25% of D?s torso over S?s torso vantaged or inferior position ay or may not be accompanied by nudging moving, standing or sitting position S must be interacting when S goes down ful mood during a play bout ly touching forelimbs to the ground with rear y towards play partnerperiod and compared matrices. Finally, to test if mean play-bout lengths were longer with preferred partners, we compared the PPP matrix with a matrix that contained mean play-bout lengths for each dyad (the total amount of time each dyad played divided by the number of play bouts for that dyad; Martin & Bateson 1993). To testwhether thenumber of stronglypreferred partners for each puppy in litter 1 increased across the next two time periods, we conducted a McNemar test in SAS version 9.1 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina, U.S.A., 2003). We also tested whether mean bout length varied by time period or dyadic sex composition (femaleefemale versus femaleemale versus maleemale) with a general linear mixed model (GLMM) in SAS (SAS Institute, 2003). We controlled for litter effects and repeated observations on dyads within a litter across time. By including litter in the model, we accounted for variables that contributed to interlitter variation. For this and all subsequent GLMMs, all two-way interaction terms were included in the initial model, but the least signi?cant interactions were sequen- tially dropped using a backwards elimination procedure. Only signi?cant interactions or those showing trend effects were included in the ?nal models. For all GLMM procedures, we tested for normality and homogeneity of variances, as appropriate, and transformed the dependent variable when necessary to meet the assumptions. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, a GLMM to determine if the degree of asymmetry differed by time or dyadic sex composition. If neither time nor sex ARTICLE IN PRESS 5composition was signi?cant, we pooled the time and sex composition data and ran a second GLMM to determine if the degree of asymmetry differed from 0. We controlled for litter effects and repeated observations on dyads within a litter across time. Relationship between play behaviours. We ran GLMMs to determine: (1) if offense behaviour rates were associated with initiation rates (dependent variable), (2) if self- handicapping rates were associated with offense behav- iour rates (dependent variable), and (3) if self-handicap- ping and offense behaviour rates (dependent variables, cf. Bauer & Smuts 2007) were associated with play bow rates. We included time as a ?xed effect, and for these and all other GLMMs that follow, we controlled for litter effects and repeated observations on puppies within a litter across time. We did not test for associations between initi- ations and self-handicapping or initiations and play bows because scatter plots suggested no relationships.Realized diversity Following Thompson (1996), we used the Shannone Weaver diversity index (Shannon & Weaver 1949) to mea- sure the diversity in choices of play partners for each puppy in all focal litters. The index (R) is R? P piln  l=pi  ln?n? where pi is the probability of a puppy initiating play with the ith puppy in the litter and n is the number of available play partners. The index ranges from 0 to 1, and R is max- imized when a puppy initiates play with all possible part- ners with equal frequency and minimized when a puppy initiates with only one partner. To test whether realized diversity scores varied by sex or time, we ran a GLMM with sex and time as ?xed effects. We controlled for litter effects and repeated observations on puppies within a litter across time periods. Asymmetries Testing the 50e50 rule. To examine whether play con- formed to the 50e50 rule, we ?rst calculated asymmetry (or conversely, symmetry) in play as follows. The number of ?wins? for individual A in a dyad (e.g. AB dyad consisting of puppies A and B) equalled the number of offense behaviours by A directed at B plus the number of self-handicapping behaviours (Table 2) by B directed towards A. B?s wins were calculated similarly. Next, we cal- culated the proportion of wins for A as the number of wins for A divided by the total number of wins for both A and B. We calculated the proportion of wins for B in the same way. We subtracted the smaller proportion of wins from the larger proportion to obtain a measure of the degree of asymmetry in each dyad. We normalized this measure with an arcsine-root transformation. The closer the value was to 0, the more symmetrical the play. Next, we ranWe calculated rates of behaviours (e.g. initiations, of- fenses, self-handicapping andplay bows) at the dyadic level Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004for each puppy in each time period as the number of times each puppy in a dyad performed one of the behaviours divided by the total time that dyad was observed playing. For example, if A and B played together for 100 s in time 1 andAdisplayed 20 offense behaviours and B 10, the offense ratewouldbe0.20 forA and0.10 for B (recall that initiations occurred only once per play boutdat the startdwhereas of- fense behaviours, self-handicapping and play bows could occurmultiple timeswithin eachbout).Weusedmean rates of behaviours for eachpuppywithin each timeperiod in the following GLMM analyses. Variation in play behaviour as a function of dyadic sex com- position. We tested whether initiation, offense, self- handicapping or play bow rates (dependent variables) varied by sex or time in mixed-sex (FM) and same-sex (FF versus MM) dyads using GLMMs as described above. We also compared rates of female behaviours when females were playing with other females (ff dyads) versus when they were playing with males (fm dyads), and similarly, we compared rates of male behaviours in maleemale (mm) dyads versus maleefemale (mf) dyads. Note that fm and mf dyads have the same composition; we use lower- versus upper-case abbreviations to remind the reader which sex?s behaviour we are measuring (the one listed ?rst in mixed-sex dyads) in the relevant analyses. For example, when measuring the rate of offense behaviours in an ?fm? dyad, we calculated only the rate for females when playing with males (see below). In contrast, when measuring the rate of offense behaviours in an FM dyad, we calculated rates for both sexes. For ff and fm dyads, we obtained difference scores for each dependent variable (e.g. rates of initiations, offense behaviours, self-handicapping andplaybows) separately by subtracting the rates of female-to-female behaviours from the rates of female-to-male behaviours. Initially, we used GLMMs to determine if difference scores varied by time. If not, we pooled observations across time periods and reran themodels to test for behavioural differences in general.We followed a similar procedure for mm and mf dyads. All statistical tests were two-tailed, unless otherwise speci?ed, and a was set at 0.05. However, if a was between 0.05 and 0.10, we report it as a nonsigni?cant trend. Frequency of offense and self-handicapping behaviours. We determined the proportions of offense and self-handicap- ping behaviours averaged across all dyads for each litter and time period as follows. First, we determined the rates of individual offense behaviours (Table 2) separately for each dyad and time period by dividing the frequency of individ- ual offense behaviours by the total time played for a given dyad. We averaged the rates of individual offense behav- iours by dyad across dyads to obtain a mean rate for each of the behaviours. Rates of self-handicapping were deter- mined similarly. We calculated the proportions of offense and self-handicapping behaviours based on mean rates. RESULTS WARD & SMUTS: PLAY AMONG DOMESTIC DOG SIBLINGSWe analysed 7.10 h of dyadic play from four litters of puppies consisting of 39 dyads and 1200 total play bouts. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, of and times combined. atrix ntly related to mean play-bout length for puppies in litters 1 ime: ntly between times 1 and 2 (P ? 0.796; Fig. 1). F2,74 ? 3.88, P ? 0.025; sex: F2,74 ? 0.510, P ? 0.605). To explore this time effect further, we ran a second GLMM pooled across sex composition while retaining time as 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1 2 3 Time periods M ea n r ea li ze d d iv er si ty s co re a b a Figure 1. Realized diversity scores across three time periods. Values are mean estimates and error bars indicate SE for litters 1 (time periods 1e3), 2 (time period 1) and 3 (time period 1). Columns with a letter in common are not significantly different (P > 0.05). Age ranges (in weeks) of puppies: time 1, 3e8; time 2, 10e23; time 3, 27e40. 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 1 2 3 Time periods D eg re e of a sy m m et ry Figure 2. The degree of asymmetry across three time periods. Values are mean estimates and error bars indicate SE for litters 1 (time periods 1e3), 2 (time periods 1 and 2), 3 (time period 1) and 4 (time period 1). To obtain these values, we ran a GLMM with the degree of asymmetry (based on nontransformed data) as the depen- dent variable and time as an independent variable. We controlled for the random effects of litter and repeated observations on dyads ARTICLE IN PRESS 6Asymmetries Testing the 50e50 rule(P ? 0.0008) and time 2 (P ? 0.004; GLMM: t F2,21 ? 8.32, P ? 0.002). Scores did not differ signi?caand 2, time 1 (rowwise matrix tests: litter 1: Kr ? 1, P ? 0.830; litter 2: Kr ? 8, P ? 0.351). However, PPP scores and mean bout length were positively related for lit- ter 1 in time 2 (rowwise matrix test: Kr ? 23, P ? 0.012) and in time 3 (Kr ? 18, P ? 0.079, trend effect). For all lit- ters combined, mean bout length varied by time period but not by dyadic sex composition (GLMM: time: F2,76 ? 4.20, P ? 0.019; dyadic sex composition: F2,76 ? 0.30, P ? 0.744). Play bouts were shorter in time 3 compared to times 1 and 2 (time 1 versus time 2: P ? 0.172; time 1 versus time 3: P ? 0.065; time 2 versus time 3: P ? 0.005; time 1: X estimate  SE ? 16.377  3.641 s; time 2: 19.934  3.966 s; time 3: 9.990  4.549 s). PPP scores in times 2 and 3 were positively correlated with scores in time 1 for litter 1 (rowwise matrix tests: times 1 and 2: Kr ? 16, P ? 0.060; times 1 and 3: Kr ? 19, P ? 0.048), but scores in times 2 and 3 were not signi?- cantly related (Kr ? 12, P ? 0.18). The number of strongly preferred partners (i.e. PPP scores  2.0) increased over time. In times 1 and 2, only one puppy from litter 1 had a strongly preferred partner (PPP scores ranged from 0 to 2.5 in time 1 and from 0 to 2.2 in time 2), but in time 3, all six puppies had one strong preference (McNemar?s test: c21 ? 5:00, P ? 0.025; PPP scores ranged from 0 to 3.76). In time 1 for litters 2 and 3, none of the puppies had strongly preferred play part- ners (PPP scores ranged from 0.32 to 1.8 for litter 2 and from 0.71 to 1.3 for litter 3), although they did appear to initiate play with particular puppies more often than with others (PPP scores > 1.0). Realized Diversity Variation in realized diversity scores was not attribut- able to sex (GLMM: F1,21 ? 0.92, P ? 0.348), but scores were lower in time 3 compared with scores in time 1P ? 0.852) or for litter 2 puppies in time 1 (rowwise m test: Kr ? 0, P ? 1.000). PPP scores were not signi?caPlay-Partner Preference Scores PPP scores were not reciprocal for puppies from litter 1 in any time period (rowwise matrix tests: time 1: Kr ? 7, P ? 0.454; time 2: Kr ? 14, P ? 0.200; time 3: Kr ? 2,13.15  5.13 bouts, and we coded an average 5.22  0.71 min of play per dyad across all littersThe average number of play bouts per dyad (X SD) was ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, --, -The overall degree of symmetry in play did not vary by sex composition, but it decreased over time (GLMM: time: Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004a ?xed effect. This analysis showed that littermates as a whole did not conform to the 50e50 rule during play in any time period (GLMM: time 1: t76 ? 7.15, P < 0.0001; time 2: t76 ? 8.52, P < 0.0001; time 3: t76 ? 7.18, P < 0.0001; all one-tailed), but symmetry was higher in time 1 relative to time 2 (P ? 0.015) and time 3 (P ? 0.024). For litter 1, there was no signi?cant differ- ence in symmetry between times 2 and 3 (P ? 0.693). Figure 2 shows the degree of asymmetry across time periods. 0.6within a litter across time. Age ranges (in weeks) of puppies: time 1, 3e8; time 2, 10e23; time 3, 27e40. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, Relationship between play behaviours The relationship between rates of initiations and offense behaviours was positive in all time periods, but it was only signi?cant in time 3 (P < 0.0001; GLMM: offense behav- iours: F1,28 ? 17.8, P ? 0.0002; time: F2,28 ? 1.92, P ? 0.165; offense behaviours*time: F2,28 ? 6.16, P ? 0.006). Neither self-handicapping nor play bows were related to offense behaviours in any time period (GLMM: self-handicapping: F1,30 ? 0.04, P ? 0.848; time: F2,30 ? 0.96, P ? 0.393; GLMM: play bows: F1,30 ? 1.35, P ? 0.254; time: F2,30 ? 1.23, P ? 0.305). However, play bows were positively associated with self-handicapping across all time periods (GLMM: play bows: F1,30 ? 9.85, P ? 0.004; time: F2,30 ? 0.40, P ? 0.677). Variation in play behaviour as a function of dyadic sex combination Rates of play initiation. In mixed-sex dyads, males initi- ated play more often than their female partners (Table 3). This result could re?ect higher rates of play initiations by males in general, but comparisons of male and female ini- tiation rates in same-sex dyads showed no differences ex- cept in time period 3, when MM rates of initiation were higher than FF rates (Table 3). Females initiated play more often with other females than with males, and results did not vary with time period (Table 3; ff versus fm dyads). However, male initiation rates did vary by time period. In time 3,males initiated playmore often with other males than with females, but in times 1 and 2, males were just as likely to initiate play with females as they were to initiate with other males (Table 3). Offense behaviour rates. In mixed-sex dyads, males dis- played offense behaviours towards females more often than the reverse (Table 4). In same-sex dyads, males and females displayed offense behaviours at similar rates across all time periods (Table 4). Rates of offense behaviours for both females and males were stable across time periods. Females displayed of- fense behaviours at similar rates whether playing with females (ff) or males (fm), but males displayed offense behaviours slightly more often when playing with females (mf) than when playing with other males (mm; trend effect; Table 4). Self-handicapping rates. In mixed-sex dyads, males self- handicapped more often than females (Table 5); however, males and females self-handicapped at similar rates in same-sex dyads (Table 5). Self-handicapping rates did not vary by sex across time periods. Females self-handicapped at similar rates whether playing with females (ff) or males (fm), and the same was true for males (Table 5). versus fm ARTICLE IN PRESS WARD & SMUTS: PLAY AMONG DOMESTIC DOG SIBLINGS 7Male initiationsz: mm versus mf Time period 1 14 1.02 0.327 Time period 2 14 0.62 0.543 Time period 3 14 2.18 0.047 mm>mf FM: femaleemale; FF: femaleefemale; MM: maleemale; ff: female rates with females; fm: female rates with males; mm: male rates with males; mf: male rates with females. T1: time 1; T2: time 2; T3: time 3. *Rates of initiations in mixed-sex and same-sex dyads in relation to sex of initiator and time period. GLMMs controlling for litter and re- peated observations of dyad{litter} across time. yTest of the difference in female initiation rates between ff and fm dyads and the difference in male initiation rates between mm and mf dyads. GLMMs controlling for litter and repeated observations of dyad{litter} across time. GLMM was pooled across time periods for female initiations owing to nonsignificant time effects (GLMM:Table 3. Initiation rates analyses Source of variation df F* or ty value P Direction of effect Mixed-sex dyads (FM) Initiator sex 1, 27 23.98 <0.0001 M>F Time period 2, 27 0.44 0.649 Same-sex dyads (FF versus MM) Initiator sex 1, 27 1.49 0.233 Time period 2, 27 5.50 0.009 T3>T1, T2 Initiator sex*time period 2, 27 4.33 0.023 MM>FF in T3 Female initiations: ff 1 15.98 0.039 ff>fmF2,9 ? 0.96, P ? 0.418). zFor male initiations, results were not pooled across time because of a trend effect with time (GLMM: F2,14 ? 2.78, P ? 0.096). Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004Play bow rates. In mixed-sex dyads, males and females play bowed at similar rates (Table 6). The same was true for females compared with males in same-sex dyads (Table 6). Play bow rates did not vary by sex across time periods. Females play bowed at similar rates whether playing with Table 4. Offense behaviour rates analyses Source of variation df F* or ty value P Direction of effect Mixed-sex dyads (FM) Sex 1, 27 7.79 0.009 M>F Time period 2, 27 0.14 0.870 Same-sex dyads (FF versus MM) Sex 1, 29 1.63 0.212 Time period 2, 29 3.21 0.055 Female offense behaviours: ff versus fm 1 0.92 0.525 Male offense behaviours: mm versus mf 2 3.19 0.086 FM: femaleemale; FF: femaleefemale; MM: maleemale; ff: female rates with females; fm: female rates with males; mm: male rates with males; mf: male rates with females. *Rates of offense behaviours in mixed-sex and same-sex dyads in relation to sex and time period. GLMMs controlling for litter and repeated observations of dyad{litter} across time. yTest of the difference in rates of offense behaviours by females in ff and fm dyads and the difference in rates by males in mm and mf dyads. GLMMs controlling for litter and repeated observations of dyad{litter} across time. GLMMs were pooled across time periods for female and male offense behaviours owing to nonsignificant time effects (GLMM: females: F2,9 ? 1.39, P ? 0.297; males: F2,14 ? 1.96, P ? 0.177). metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, were , b). Table 5. Self-handicapping rates analyses Source of variation df F* or ty value P Direction of effect Mixed-sex dyads (FM) Self-handicapper sex 1, 27 4.87 0.036 M>F Time period 2, 27 0.04 0.960 Same-sex dyads (FF versus MM) Self-handicapper sex 1, 29 0.13 0.723 Time period 2, 29 0.92 0.442 Female self-handicapping: ff versus fm 1 1.17 0.449 Male self-handicapping: mm versus mf 2 1.01 0.418 FM: femaleemale; FF: femaleefemale; MM: maleemale; ff: female rates with females; fm: female rates with males; mm: male rates with males; mf: male rates with females. *Self-handicapping rates in mixed-sex and same-sex dyads in rela- tion to sex and time period. GLMMs controlling for litter and re- peated observations of dyad{litter} across time. GLMMs were pooled across time periods for female and male self-handicapping ARTICLE IN PRESS ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, --, -8behaviours owing to nonsignificant time effects (GLMM: females: F2,9 ? 0.51, P ? 0.615; males: F2,14 ? 1.35, P ? 0.291).peated observations of dyad{litter} across time. yTest of the difference in female self-handicapping rates between ff and fm dyads and the difference in male self-handicapping rates be- tween mm and mf dyads. GLMMs controlling for litter and re-females (ff) or males (fm), and the same was true for males (Table 6). Frequency of offense and self-handicapping behaviours Rates of individual offense and self-handicapping be- haviours were similar across litters, so we pooled the data et of in- Table 6. Play bow rates analyses Source of variation df F* or ty value P Mixed-sex dyads (FM) Sex 1, 27 0.80 0.379 Time period 2, 27 2.24 0.126 Same-sex dyads (FF versus MM) Sex 1, 29 2.29 0.141 Time period 2, 29 1.47 0.246 Female play bows: ff versus fm 1 0.88 0.542 Male play bows: mm versus mf 2 0.78 0.516 FM: femaleemale; FF: femaleefemale; MM: maleemale; ff: female rates with females; fm: female rates with males; mm: male rates with males; mf: male rates with females. *Play bow rates in mixed-sex and same-sex dyads in relation to sex and time period. GLMMs controlling for litter and repeated obser- vations of dyad{litter} across time. yTest of the difference in female play bow rates between ff and fm dyads and the difference in male play bow rates between mm and mf dyads. GLMMs controlling for litter and repeated observa- tions of dyad{litter} across time. GLMMs were pooled across time periods for female and male play bows owing to nonsignificant time effects (GLMM: females: F2,9 ? 0.77, P ? 0.490; males: F2,14 ? 2.01, P ? 0.171). Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004creased over time as diversity in partner choice decreased, and by time 3, each puppy had one strongly preferred play partner. PPP scores were consistent between early and late time periods for litter 1. These results indicate that puppies begin to form differentiated relationships very early in life. Detailed information on play partner preferences in other animals is rare, but preferences for speci?c play partners occur in infant sable antelope, Hippotragus niger (Thompson 1996) and juvenile Belding?s ground squirrels, Spermophilus beldingi (Nunes et al. 2004). Like the littermates in this study, sable calves played with a variety of partners early on, but later (9e12 weeks) developed stable preferences with a subset of strongly preferred playmates. However, unlike young sable antelope, puppies did not show recip- rocal play partner preferences, a ?nding we discuss later. Asymmetries Testing the 50e50 rule Puppies did not adhere to the 50e50 rule in any time period; however, play was more symmetrical between very young littermates and became less so as the puppies matured. Red fox littermates of approximately 4e8 weeks of age (three litters consisting of two trios and one pair) also showed considerable asymmetry during social playconcentrated on initiating play with a smaller subs individuals. Their preferences for speci?c partnersAlso in time 2, mounts appeared for the ?rst time (5% of offense plus self-handicapping behaviours in both litters), and the proportion of bite shakes decreased (Fig. 3b) In time 3, mounts became the most common offense behav- iour, followed by chases, and bite shakes disappeared entirely (Fig. 3c). Muzzle bites and chin overs (Table 2) were a small proportion of offense behaviours across all time periods. In time 1, nearly 100% of self-handicapping behaviours involved voluntary downs (Fig. 3a; Table 2), but by time 2, muzzle licks had grown to 44% of all self-handicapping behaviours (in addition to voluntary downs; Fig. 3b). In time 3, 75% of self-handicapping was accounted for by muzzle licks (performed by one female; Fig. 3c). DISCUSSION Partner Preferences and Diversity In time periods 1 and 2, puppies in all focal litters initiated play with most of the other individuals in their litter. However, as they matured, puppies in litter 1In times 1 and 2, forced downs and overs (Table 2) the two most common offense behaviours (Fig. 3aacross litters and report results by time period. Offense behaviours were much more common than self-handicap- ping behaviours across all time periods, accounting for 94% of all behaviours (offense and self-handicapping behaviours combined; Table 2) during time 1 (Fig. 3a), 91% of behaviours during time 2 (Fig. 3b) and 84% of be- haviours during time 3 (Fig. 3c).(the proportion of ?wins? by the fox who won most often within each dyad varied from 62 to 82%; values derived metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, MVoluntary down bite Mount 5% ARTICLE IN PRESS 9Bite shake 13% Chin over 2% Chase 8% Forced down 36% Over 35% Chin over 1% Forced down 36% Chase 12% Over 33% Muzzle <1% (a) (b)from Table 2; Meyer & Weber 1996). Bauer & Smuts (2007) reported that adult dogs did not conform to the 50e50 rule during dyadic play, and they found that play in adult dogs re?ects the existing dominance structure outside of play. The same is true for adult meerkats (Sharpe 2005a). The studies mentioned above, however, did not pro- vide data on changes in the degree of asymmetry over time. In young rats paired together, symmetry in play decreased over time as stable dominance relationships developed (Panksepp 1981). Like rats (Panksepp 1981; Pellis & Pellis 1991), dogs may use play to establish stable social relationships and test their place in the existing so- cial structure of a group (Bekoff 1972). If so, then the in- creased asymmetry over time among littermates may simply re?ect dominance relationships formed during the juvenile period, making puppy play more like that of adult dogs in this respect (cf. Bauer & Smuts 2007). Domestic dogs, like wild meerkats (Sharpe 2005a), do not use aggression (distinct from play ?ghting) to estab- lish stable dominance relationships in the ?rst few weeks of life, in contrast to some other social carnivores (cap- tive coyotes: Bekoff 1974; spotted hyaenas, Crocuta Bite shake 4% Force 1 Chin over 1% Chase 27% Over 6% Muzzle bite 1% Mount 37% (c) Figure 3. Proportions of all offense and self-handicapping behaviours for ( 1). Large pie includes offense behaviours and self-handicapping; small p ranges (in weeks) of puppies: time 1, 3e8; time 2, 10e23; time 3, 27e Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004Self- handicapping 9% Muzzle lick 5%uzzle bite <1% Muzzle lick <1% Voluntary down 6% Self- handicapping 6% WARD & SMUTS: PLAY AMONG DOMESTIC DOG SIBLINGScrocuta: Wahaj & Holekamp 2006). As far as we know, re- searchers have not addressed the functional signi?cance of interspeci?c variation in the presence or absence of early ?ghting to establish stable dominance relationships within litters. Relationships between play behaviours In time period 3, rates of initiations were directly related to rates of offense behaviours. This ?nding suggests that as puppies mature, winning or being in the dominant position during play becomes more important. Similarly, young male squirrel monkeys (Biben 1998), baboons (Owens 1975), and laboratory rats (Hole 1988) preferred partners they could ?dominate? during play. It seems likely that in dogs, as in rats, male bison Bison bison, and many primates, individuals become increasingly aware of intra- group status as they mature, and play becomes more com- petitive (Fagen 1981; Rothstein & Griswold 1991; Smith et al. 1999). Preference for partners who can be dominated could help explain why PPP scores were not reciprocal (see above). Self- handicapping 16% 4% Voluntary down 4% d down 2% Muzzle lick 12% a) time 1 (litters 1e4), (b) time 2 (litters 1 and 2) and (c) time 3 (litter ie shows proportions of specific self-handicapping behaviours. Age 40. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, ing were less successful than mating attempts that elicited female cooperation (Ghosh et al. 1984; Pal et al. 1999). In same-sex dogs (Borchelt 1983; Sherman et al. 1996), same- sex partners may play to practice threat and appeasement ARTICLE IN PRESS 10summary, play with females may allow males to practice both cooperative and competitive mating strategies in a less serious context. In contrast to our results, Bauer & Smuts (2007) found no sex differences in attacks/pursuits (cf. offense behav- iours this study) or self-handicapping. Possible discrep- ancies in the ?ndings may be related to the choice of subjects. Bauer & Smuts (2007) studied unrelated adultSimilar to the ?ndings of Bauer & Smuts (2007) for adult dogs, we found no association between offense behaviours and either self-handicapping or play bows, but high rates of self-handicapping were related to high rates of play bows across all time periods. This suggests that self- handicapping and play signalling may function together to encourage play (Bauer & Smuts 2007). Variation in play behaviour as a function of dyadic sex combination Mixed-sex partners. In mixed-sex dyads, males initiated play more often than females across all time periods (Table 3). With one possible exception (MM dyads initiated more often than FF dyads in time 3), these results were not just an artefact of males being more active in general. Similar to our ?ndings, Lund & Vestergaard (1998) reported that males initiated social play with females more often than expected by chance in four litters of domestic dogs ages 3 to 8 weeks. Male free-ranging dog puppies studied in India engaged in social play more frequently than female puppies; however, it is not clear whether these male puppies were playing with other males, other females or both (Pal 2008). In addition to initiating at higher rates than females, males in FM dyads also displayed self-handicapping more often than females and, as indicated above, self-handicap- ping correlates with play signalling and appears to function to maintain play. Perhaps playing with females provides opportunities for males to learn characteristics of female behaviour and gain competence in interactions with them. If so, it could translate into greater male reproductive success later in life via female mate prefer- ences. Pal et al. (1999) found that in a population of free- ranging dogs in India, oestrous females selectively allowed some males to mate and even solicited them but avoided other males or even attacked them if they tried to mount. Beach & LeBoeuf (1967) also reported female mating pref- erences in captive dogs. In FM dyads, male puppies displayed offense behaviours more often than females (Table 4). In food competition tests, male puppies also tended to dominate females by 15 weeks (Scott & Fuller 1965). Learning to dominate fe- males may have reproductive payoffs as well. For example, in Indian free-ranging dogs, males forced copulations on unwilling, often sexually inexperienced, oestrous females who responded with a combination of aggressive, submis- sive, and escape postures. However, attempts to force mat- ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, --, -dogs, whereas we examined social relationships between young littermates. Ways in which age and kinship might Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004signals that ritualize aggression. Practicing aggressive com- ponents within a safe range of intensity could limit the occurrence of overt aggression later on (Lindsay 2005). In adult pet dogs, ?ghts between females lead to more se- rious injuries than ?ghts between males or ?ghts between mixed-sex dogs (Sherman et al. 1996). The early onset and consistent preference for females to initiate play with other females may be one way females learn to moderate same-sex aggression. In contrast to some of our ?ndings, Lund & Vestergaard (1998) reported that male and female puppies (littermates up to 8 weeks of age) did not prefer same- over mixed-sex play partners. However, Lund & Vestergaard (1998) did not limit their observations to dyadic interactions, as in the current study, but also included triadic interactions, which could have in?uenced their ?ndings in ways that remain to be investigated. Additionally, the increased rates of same-sex initiations recorded in our study were not simply an artefact of play- bout lengths varying by dyadic sex combination (e.g. males initiated with males more often than they initiated with females in time 3 because MM play-bout lengths were shorter than FM bout lengths in time 3; Table 3). Mean bout lengths did not vary by dyadic sex composi- tion across any time period in the current study. Preferences for play with same-sex partners have been reported in other species, including juvenile male bison (Rothstein & Griswold 1991), rats of both sexes (Laviola & Terranova 1998) and juvenile male Belding?s ground squir- rels (Nunes et al. 2004). Individual offense and self-handicapping behaviours Some behaviours were more common early in develop- ment (e.g. forced downs and overs) and became less common as puppies matured. Conversely, other behav- iours were less common early in development but became more common with time (e.g. mounts). These ?ndings suggest that the style and possibly the function of play, even between the same play partners, may change over time. This is clearly the case for male (but not female) rats, who, at puberty, shift the type of defensive strategy used with other males as they begin to establish dominance relationships (Smith et al. 1996). It is not clear, however,in?uence intersexual play behaviours remain to be investigated. Same-sex partners. The preference for same-sex play initiations in males and females during various time periods (Table 3) suggests that play may serve as training for intrasexual competition between same-sex littermates. Like wolves (Mech 1970; Packard 2003), both female and male domestic dogs form intrasexual dominance relation- ships (Pal et al. 1998), and play may function in the for- mation of these dominance relationships (Bekoff 1972). Because dominance con?icts generally occur betweenwhether puppies show sharp discontinuities in frequen- cies of different play behaviours, as is the case for rats metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, ARTICLE IN PRESS 11(Smith et al. 1996) and domestic cats (Barrett & Bateson 1978; Caro 1981), but future work could address this area. Even with a small sample of four litters comprising various breeds, litter sizes, and sex ratios, we found consistent patterns of partner preferences, role asymme- tries, and sex differences (for example, in rates of offense behaviours). These results suggest that domestic dog littermates, in general, show important similarities in social development. It would be interesting to determine whether these general patterns evolved during domesti- cation or were inherited from wolf ancestors, but compa- rable data are not available for wolves. Despite the similarities we found across litters, studies of other species suggest that important interlitter variations in play behaviour are likely to occur. Play behaviour varies among litters as a function of litter size, litter sex ratio, and nutritional state or weight (Caro 1981; Laviola & Alleva 1995; D?Eath & Lawrence 2004; Nunes et al. 2004). Such interlitter variability is also correlated with major differ- ences in adult behaviours, including aggressiveness, sexual behaviour, maternal behaviour and behavioural, hor- monal and neurochemical responses to stress and novelty (Sharpe et al. 1973; Namikas & Wehmer 1978; Laviola & Terranova 1998; D?Eath & Lawrence 2004). Experimental studies in rodents manipulating these variables show that in at least some cases, interlitter variation in size, sex ratios and play-?ghting behaviours are not only correlated with, but actually cause, differences in adult behaviours (Laviola & Terranova 1998; Pellis & Pellis 2007). It is no surprise that social interactions early in life affect adult behaviour. However, we know very little about why particular early experiences affect adult behaviour in the ways they do. Most of the data relating developmental variation to adult behaviour come from laboratory rodents or domestic piglets reared in environments different from those of their wild counterparts, which limits our ability to analyse these relationships from an adaptive perspec- tive. We think that domestic dogs afford an exceptional opportunity to pursue such questions because different breeds have undergone systematic selection for adult dif- ferences in behaviour (including social behaviour, e.g. hounds and sled dogs must be able to work peacefully with conspeci?cs in groups and terriers, as rodent hunters, tend to work as individuals). Future research on domestic dog littermates can help to: (1) reveal how speci?c selec- tion pressures alter early social experiences and (2) explain why some aspects of social development remain similar across litters and across breeds. Such studies are likely to advance our theoretical understanding of relationships between evolutionary and ontogenetic processes. Acknowledgments We thank Monika Dressler, Lynne Coleman, Mary West- off, Mara Markov and Pamela Fusco for allowing us to videotape their litters. We thank Kathy Welch for her statistical consultation, numerous undergraduate students for assistance with data collection and coding (especially Jane Na and Sarah Alessi) and Theresa Lee, Bobbi Low, Patricia McConnell and John Mitani for their reviews of the manuscript. This study was supported by funding Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004from the Horace H. Rackham School of Graduate Studies at the University of Michigan, the Psychology Department at the University of Michigan, and the Center for the Education of Women at the University of Michigan. The research presented here was approved by the University Committee on the Use and Care of Animals at the University of Michigan, Approval 8988. References Abrantes, R. 1997. Dog Language. Napierville, Illinois: Wakan Tanka Publishers. Aldis, O. 1975. Playfighting. New York: Academic Press. Altmann, J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227e267. Balfour, A. D. 1987. Social relationships in litters of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Ph.D. thesis, University of Guelph. Barrett, P. & Bateson, P. 1978. The development of play in cats. Behaviour, 66, 106e120. Bauer, E. B. & Smuts, B. B. 2007. Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Animal Behaviour, 73, 489e499. Beach, F. A. & LeBoeuf, B. J. 1967. Coital behaviour in dogs. I. Preferential mating in the bitch. Animal Behaviour, 15, 546e558. Bekoff, M. 1972. Development of social interaction, play, and metacommunication in mammals: an ethological perspective. Quarterly Review of Biology, 47, 412e434. Bekoff, M. 1974. Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. American Zoologist, 14, 323e340. Bekoff, M. 1984. Social play behavior. Bioscience, 34, 228e233. Bekoff, M. 1995. Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids. Behaviour, 132, 419e429. Bekoff, M. 2001. Social play behavior: cooperation, fairness, trust, and the evolution of morality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 81e90. Bekoff, M. & Allen, C. 1998. Intentional communication and social play: how and why animals negotiate and agree to play. In: Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives (Ed. by M. Bekoff & J. A. Byers), pp. 97e114. Cambridge, U.K.: Cam- bridge University Press. Bekoff, M. & Byers, J. A. 1981. A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: an ethological hornet?s nest. In: Behavioral Development: the Bielefeld Interdisciplinary Pro- ject (Ed. by K. Immelmann, G. W. Barlow & L. Petrinovich), pp. 296e337. New York: Cambridge University Press. Biben, M. 1983. Comparative ontogeny of social behaviour in three South-American canids, the maned wolf, crab-eating fox and bush dogdimplications for sociality. Animal Behaviour, 31, 814e826. Biben, M. 1986. Individual- and sex-related strategies of wrestling play in captive squirrel monkeys. Ethology, 71, 229e241. Biben, M. 1998. Squirrel monkey play-fighting: making the case for a cognitive training function for play. In: Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives (Ed. by M. Bekoff & J. A. Byers), pp. 161e182. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Borchelt, P. L. 1983. Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as compan- ion animals: classification and influence of sex, reproductive status and breed. Applied Animal Ethology, 10, 45e61. Burghardt, G. M. 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Byers, J. A. 1998. Biological effects of locomotor play: getting into shape, or something more specific? In: Animal Play: Evolutionary, WARD & SMUTS: PLAY AMONG DOMESTIC DOG SIBLINGSComparative, and Ecological Perspectives (Ed. by M. Bekoff & J. A. Byers), pp. 205e220. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, ARTICLE IN PRESS ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, --, -12Caro, T. M. 1981. Sex differences in the termination of social play in cats. Animal Behaviour, 29, 271e279. D?Eath, R. B. & Lawrence, A. B. 2004. Early life predictors of the development of aggressive behaviour in the pig. Animal Behaviour, 67, 501e509. Derix, R., van Hooff, J., de Vries, H. & Wensing, J. 1993. Male and female mating competition in wolves: female suppression vs. male intervention. Behaviour, 127, 141e174. Fagen, R.1981.Animal Play Behavior. NewYork:OxfordUniversity Press. Fagen, R. & Fagen, J. 2004. Juvenile survival and benefits of play behaviour in brown bears, Ursus arctos. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 6, 89e102. Freedman, D. G., Elliot, O. & King, J. A. 1961. Critical period in social development of dogs. Science, 133, 1016e1017. Ghosh, B., Choudhuri, D. K. & Pal, B. 1984. Some aspects of the sexual behaviour of stray dogs, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 13, 113e127. Hemelrijk, C. K. 1990a. Models of, and tests for, reciprocity, unidir- ectionality and other social interaction patterns at a group level. Animal Behaviour, 39, 1013e1029. Hemelrijk, C. K. 1990b. A matrix partial correlation test used in investigations of reciprocity and other social interaction patterns at group level. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 143, 405e420. Hole, G. 1988. Temporal features of social play in the laboratory rat. Ethology, 78, 1e20. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. & Wensing, J. A. B. 1987. Dominance and its behavioral measures in a captive wolf pack. In: Man and Wolf (Ed. by H. Frank), pp. 219e252. Dordrecht: Dr W. Junk Publishers. Laviola, G. & Alleva, E. 1995. Sibling effects on the behavior of infant mouse litters (Mus domesticus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 68e75. Laviola, G. & Terranova, M. L. 1998. The developmental psychobi- ology of behavioural plasticity in mice: the role of social experi- ences in the family unit. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 197e213. Lindsay, S. R. 2000. Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training: Adaptation and Learning. Ames: Iowa State University Press. Lindsay, S. R. 2005. Applied Dog Behavior and Training: Procedures and Protocols. Ames, Iowa: Blackwell Scientific. Lund, J. D. & Vestergaard, K. S. 1998. Development of social behaviour in four litters of dogs (Canis familiaris). Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica, 39, 183e193. McLeod, P. J. & Fentress, J. C. 1997. Developmental changes in the sequential behavior of interacting timber wolf pups. Behavioural Processes, 39, 127e136. Maestripieri, D. & Ross, S. R. 2004. Sex differences in play among western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) infants: implications for adult behavior and social structure. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 123, 52e61. Martin, P. & Bateson, P. 1993. Measuring Behaviour. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Mech, L. D. 1970. The Wolf: the Ecology and Behaviour of an Endan- gered Species. New York: Doubleday. Meyer, S. & Weber, J. 1996. Ontogeny of dominance in free-living foxes. Ethology, 102, 1008e1019. Namikas, J. & Wehmer, G. 1978. Gender composition of the litter affects behaviour of male mice. Behavioral Biology, 23, 219e224. Nunes, S., Muecke, E., Sanchez, Z., Hoffmeier, R. R. & Lancaster, L. T. 2004. Play behavior and motor development in juvenile Belding?s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 97e105.Owens, N. W. 1975. Social play behaviour in free-living baboons, Papio anubis. Animal Behaviour, 23, 387e408. Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004Packard, J. M. 2003. Wolf behavior: reproductive, social, and intelli- gent. In: Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation (Ed. by L. D. Mech& L. Boitani), pp. 35e65. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Pal, S. K. 2008. Maturation and development of social behaviour during early ontogeny in free ranging dog puppies in West Bengal, India. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 111, 95e107. Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B. & Roy, S. 1998. Agonistic behaviour of free- ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to season, sex and age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 59, 331e348. Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B. & Roy, S. 1999. Inter- and intra-sexual behaviour of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 62, 267e278. Palagi, E., Cordoni, G. & Borgognini, T. 2004. Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpan- zees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology, 110, 949e962. Panksepp, J. 1981. The ontogeny of play in rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 14, 327e332. Pellis, S. M. & Pellis, V. C. 1991. Role reversal changes during the ontogeny of play fighting in male rats: attack vs. defense. Aggres- sive Behavior, 17, 179e189. Pellis, S. M. & Pellis, V. C. 1998. Structureefunction interface in the analysis of play. In: Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Eco- logical Perspectives (Ed. by M. Bekoff & J. A. Byers), pp. 115e140. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Pellis, S. M. & Pellis, V. C. 2007. Rough-and-tumble play and the development of the social brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 95e98. Pereira, M. E. & Preisser, M. C. 1998. Do strong primate players ?self-handicap? during competitive social play? Folia Primatologica, 69, 177e180. Pozis-Francois, O., Zahavi, A. & Zahavi, A. 2004. Social play in Arabian babblers. Behaviour, 141, 425e450. Rothstein, A. & Griswold, J. G. 1991. Age and sex preferences for social partners by juvenile bison bulls, Bison bison. Animal Behav- iour, 41, 227e237. Schenkel, R. 1967. Submission: its features and function in wolf and dog. American Zoologist, 7, 319e329. Scott, J. P. & Fuller, J. L. 1965. Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Scott, J. P. & Marston, M. 1950. Critical periods affecting the devel- opment of normal and mal-adjustive social behavior of puppies. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 77, 25e60. Shannon, C. E. & Weaver, W. 1949. The Mathematical Theory of Communication. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Sharpe, L. L. 2005a. Play fighting does not affect subsequent fighting success in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1023e1029. Sharpe, L. L. 2005b. Play does not enhance social cohesion in a cooperative mammal. Animal Behaviour, 70, 551e558. Sharpe, L. L. 2005c. Frequency of social play does not affect dispersal partnerships in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 70, 559e569. Sharpe, R. M., Morris, A. & Wyatt, A. C. 1973. The effect of the sex of littermates on the subsequent behaviour and breeding perfor- mance of cross-fostered rats. Laboratory Animals, 7, 51e59. Sherman, C. K., Reisner, I. R., Taliaferro, L. A. & Houpt, K. A. 1996. Characteristics, treatment, and outcome of 99 cases of aggression between dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 47, 91e108. Smith, L. K., Field, E. F., Forgia, M. L. & Pellis, S. M. 1996. Dom- inance and age-related changes in the play-fighting of intact and post-weaning castrated male rats (Rattus norvegicus). Aggressive Behavior, 22, 215e226. Smith, L. K., Fantella, S. L. N. & Pellis, S. M. 1999. Playful defensive responses in adult male rats depend on the status of the unfamiliar opponent. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 141e152. metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates, Spinka, M., Newberry, R. C. & Bekoff, M. 2001. Mammalian play: training for theunexpected.Quarterly Reviewof Biology,76, 141e168. Thompson, K. V. 1996. Play-partner preferences and the function of social play in infant sable antelope, Hippotragus niger. Animal Behaviour, 52, 1143e1155. Thompson, K. V. 1998. Self-assessment in juvenile play. In: Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives (Ed. by M. Bekoff & J. A. Byers), pp. 183e204. Cambridge, U.K.: Cam- bridge University Press. deVries,H.1993. The rowwise correlationbetween twoproximityma- trices and the partial rowwise correlation. Psychometrika, 58, 53e69. Wahaj, S. A. & Holekamp, K. E. 2006. Functions of sibling aggres- sion in the spotted hyaena, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour, 71, 1401e1409. Watson, D. M. & Croft, D. B. 1996. Age-related differences in play- fighting strategies of captive male red-necked wallabies (Macropus rufogriseus banksianus). Ethology, 102, 336e346. West, M. 1974. Social play in the domestic cat. American Zoologist, 14, 427e436. Zahavi, A. 1977. Testing of a bond. Animal Behaviour, 25, 246e247. ARTICLE IN PRESS WARD & SMUTS: PLAY AMONG DOMESTIC DOG SIBLINGS 13Please cite this article in press as: Camille Ward et al., Partner preferences and asym Anim. Behav. (2008), doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004metries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates,