A tropical horde of counterfeit predator eyes Daniel H. Janzen a,1 , Winnie Hallwachs a , and John M. Burns b a Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6018; and b Department of Entomology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC 20013-7012 Edited by May R. Berenbaum, University of Illinois at Urbana?Champaign, Urbana, IL, and approved May 14, 2010 (received for review January 26, 2010) Weproposethatthemanydifferent,butessentiallysimilar,eye-likeandface-likecolorpatternsdisplayedbyhundredsofspeciesoftropical caterpillars and pupae?26 examples of which are displayed here from the dry, cloud, and rain forests of Area de ConservacionGuanacaste (ACG) in northwestern Costa Rica?constitute a huge and pervasive mimicry complex that is evolutionarily generated and sustained by the survival behavior of a large and multispeci?c array of potential predators: the insect-eating birds. We propose that these predators are variously and innately programmed to ?ee when abruptly confronted, at close range, with what appears to be an eye of one of their predators. Such a mimetic complex differs from various classical Batesian and M?llerian mimicry complexes of adult butter?ies in that (i) the predators sustain it for the most part by innate traits rather than by avoidance behavior learned through disagreeable experiences, (ii) the more or less harmless, sessile, and largely edible mimics vastly outnumber the models, and (iii) there is no particular selection for the eye-like color pattern to closely mimic the eye or face of any particular predator of the insect-eating birds or that of any other member of this mimicry complex. Indeed, selection may not favor exact resemblance among these mimics at all. Such convergence through selection could create a superabundance of one particular false eyespot or face pattern, thereby increasing the likelihood of a bird species or guild learning to associate that pattern with harmless prey. caterpillar | mimicry | pupa | insectivorous birds | innate behavior Y ou are a 12-gram, insectivorous, tropical rainforest bird, foraging in shady, tangled, dappled, rus- tling foliage where edible cater- pillars and other insects are likely to shelter. You want to live 10?20 years. You are peering under leaves, poking into rolled ones, searching around stems, ex- ploring bark crevices and other insect hiding places. Abruptly an eye appears, 1?5 centimeters from your bill. The eye or a portion of it is half seen, obstructed, shadowed, partly out of focus, more or less round, multicolored, and perhaps moving. If you pause a millisecond to ask whether that eye belongs to acceptable prey or to a predator, you are likely to be?and it takes only once?someone?s breakfast. Your innate reaction to the eye must be instant ?ight, that is, a ?startle? coupled with distancing. The bird that must learn to avoid what appears to be a predator?s eye is not long for this world. Now, a safe few meters away, are you going to go back to see whether that was food? No. You, like billions of other individuals and hundreds of other species for tens of millions of years, have just been a player in an act of natural selection favoring mutations that lead to the multitudes Fig. 1. The 7-mm-wide pupa of Cephise nuspesez (23) (Hesperiidae), a Costa Rican skipper butter?yasit appears to a foraging bird that (Upper) has poked into the front of the rolled leaf shelter constructed by the caterpillar or (Lower) has opened the roll from above. When disturbed, this pupa rotates to present its face to the open end of the leaf roll. Author contributions: D.H.J., W.H., and J.M.B. designed re- search; D.H.J., W.H., and J.M.B. performed research; D.H.J., W.H., and J.M.B. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; D.H.J., W.H., and J.M.B. analyzed data; and D.H.J., W.H., and J.M.B. wrote the paper. The authors declare no con?ict of interest. This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. 1 To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: djanzen@sas.upenn.edu. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas. 0912122107/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0912122107 PNAS Early Edition | 1of7 PERSPECTIVE of ?false eye? color patterns, ?eyespot? patterns, or ?facsimiles of eyes? and ?faces? adorning tropical caterpillars and pupae (Figs. 1?4, and SI Appendix, Figs. S1 and S2). These eyespots are round or oval and mono- or polychromatic, with round or slit pupils. Associated body pat- terns often suggest other head and facial features, which in turn enhance the eye-like nature of the spots. Depending on the angle of observation and on how much or which part shows, one pattern may even simulate different faces (Figs. 1 and 2). None of these patterns exactly matches the eyes or face of any particular species of predator; but, even when quickly and partially glimpsed, all give the illusion of an eye or face. These false eyes are mimicking the eyes and faces of such predators of insect-eating birds as snakes, lizards, other birds, and small mammals, as perceived at close range by the in- sectivorous birds in their natural world. These color patterns?long noticed by ?eld naturalists, evolutionary behaviorists (see especially refs. 1?3), ecologists, tax- onomists, ecotourists, and, no doubt, our distant ancestors?and the birds? reactions to them, are the evolutionary footprints of predator/prey encounters as shallow as today and as deep as the ?rst terrestrial vertebrate eyes. Such footprints are scat- tered across many diurnal vertebrate/prey interactions (e.g., refs. 4 and 5), but here we focus only on those of caterpillars and pupae and the birds that eat them. Discussion We postulate that both the frequent oc- currence of false eyespots on tropical cat- erpillars and pupae, and the great tax- onomic diversity of their bearers, are powerful indirect evidence that the avian reactions to false eyes are innate. Never- theless, we expect the reactions to vary interspeci?cally in connection with the intensity of the bird?s selective regime, the bird?s learning ability and personal history of predator avoidance, and the evoca- tiveness (e.g., ref. 5) of any particular false eye(s) as perceived by the bird in the habitats in which it characteristically forages. The response may also vary in- traspeci?cally with the bird?s microenvi- ronmental circumstances and with experience?for example, light level, proximity, degree to which the false eye or the whole insect is obstructed, what the bird?s neighbors and life have taught it, whether it has recently suffered a near miss, how hungry it is (e.g., refs. 3, 6), size of eyespot (e.g., refs. 5, 7), etc. The sum of these avian reactions across many tropical circumstances, habitats, and ecosystems is a diffuse selective pressure to which we suggest that innumerable species of caterpillars and pupae have variously responded in the evolution of their color patterns. The diffuse nature of the syndrome highlighted here also applies to mimicry based on aposematic (warning) colors, cryptic colors, ?ash col- ors, and behaviors associated with them. For example, the outcome of innate avoidance of coral snakes by birds (8) may extend to other toxic snakes and to harm- less ones (4), to turtles (9), and to both toxic and harmless caterpillars (9). Those species-rich tropical complexes (4) are not our concern here but seem to display the same phenomenon with the same root cause. The 36 species of caterpillars and pupae in Figs. 1?4 and SI Appendix, Figs. S1 and S2, are a small and partial repre- sentation of the hundreds of species with false eyes and faces encountered in the course of a 30-year, ongoing inventory of ca. 450,000 individuals and >5,000 species of caterpillars and pupae in the dry, cloud, and rain forest of Area de Con- servacion Guanacaste (ACG) in north- western Costa Rica (http://janzen.sas. upenn.edu; ref. 10). Each of these species of immature mothsorbutter?ies has its own evolu- tionary pedigree. Each has its own degree of retention of traits that have been in- tensively favored by natural selection in the past and that may not be maintained today by anything more complex than phylogenetic inertia, the absence of an opposing selective force (11), and the multispeci?c array of insect-eating birds scouring tropical vegetation every day, year in and year out. Once a species has evolved false eyes (or any facial pattern that elicits a fear/?ee reaction), those false eyes may barely diminish crypticity Fig. 2. The 50-mm-long last instar caterpillar of Costa Rican Ridens panche (Hesperiidae) at the moment when its leaf shelter is forced open (Upper) and a few seconds later (Lower), when it presents glowing red false eye spots directed at the invader and glowing lemon-yellow eye spots in the dark of the cavern behind. Both kinds of false eyes are thrust at the leaf roll entrance until the invader leaves. 2of7 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0912122107 Janzen et al. at a distance, cost next to nothing physi- ologically, and greatly bene?t their bearer in a close encounter with a predator. These color patterns differ from those of classic mimic/model systems in that their value to the mimic depends not on the closeness of the match to a speci?c model, but rather on suf?cient similarity to an eye and/or face to trigger the fear/?ee reaction in an insect-eating bird. How similar is necessary (5) will vary from bird species to bird species and among foraging situations. The ubiquity of this multi- speci?c mimicry complex argues strongly for the widespread presence of the fear/ ?ee reaction in many species of small birds (or a few quite common ones). Much contemporary mimicry theory and popular commentary explicitly or implicitly stress the importance of experience, learning, and memory of the potential predator in predator/prey interactions. However, we believe that it generally underestimates the fact that potential predators also innately avoid various apo- sematicsignalsandsimilaritiestoattributes of their predators, as shown by both ex- perimental study (e.g., refs. 5, 8, 12?16), observation (see especially ref. 4), and our natural history observations, and as sketched out by Blest (3) although sub- sequently largely ignored for the past half century. When the dominant response of the predator is innate rather than (or as well as) learned, there are major changes in mimicry theory and interpretation of natural history with respect to the relative importance of mimic/model ratios (4), scarcity of models (4, 17, 18), intensity of selection (5, 15), ability to remember (6), etc. Any color, pattern, motion, or sound of a caterpillar or pupa that elicits innate avoidance of a lethal outcome for the bird selectively favors both the preda- tor and the prey. False eye and face mimicry need not ?exactly? match the real eyes of any particular species of predator in order to be selected for, much as highly effective cryptic behavior and color pat- terns often do not precisely match the patterns and colors of any particular background. The eyespot and face pat- terns need only contain features that stimulate predator recognition by small predators themselves (5). Although Blest (ref. 3 and references therein) built on these concepts in detail, they have re- ceived little attention from the many bi- ologists dealing with mimic/model systems in the tropics, most of whom have focused on the exactness of mimicry among distasteful models and mimics that display diurnally in ostentatious ?ight. False-eye color patterns on butter?y wings may serve to de?ect a bird?s strike from the actual head of the butter?y (3, 19, 20) instead of startling a potential predator away. However, false-eye color patterns on butter?y wings can also reduce predation attempts (refs. 5 and 16 and reviews therein). Both of these hypothe- sized and con?rmed processes may be operative at the same time with the same species of prey and different species of predators, but we are concerned here with false eyes and faces on relatively sessile caterpillars and pupae. There is no selec- tive value in de?ecting a bird?s strike to the site of the false eye on these animals. Equally, we are not concerned here with the question of what shape or intensity of an eyespot (5) confers protection at any given moment with any particular bird. Some ?rst and approximate general- izations about the mimic/model complex of tropical caterpillars and pupae (Figs. 1?4 and SI Appendix, Figs. S1?S14) emerge from our observations of their natural history. Taken as a whole, their traits suggest to us the long-term and pervasive operation of natural selection by the species-rich and abundant guild of small vertebrate diurnal predators on cater- pillars and pupae in tropical forest. We do not propose alternative hypotheses for this multispeci?c display by sessile prey because we cannot think of any that are compatible with the collective natural history of the hundreds of species of avian and lepidopteran actors. False eyes and faces: (i) Are comparatively more common on species of caterpillars and pupae that live in great part concealed in microhabitats that are often of low and variable light levels, and that are searched by diurnally foraging birds?rolled and silked leaves, silk/ leaf/dead leaf tangles, dark shadows under large leaves, crevices in tree bark, etc. Because many kinds of in- sects and spiders hide in such pla- ces, they are rich foraging grounds for birds?but with hidden dangers. Consider the caterpillar of the hes- periid (skipper) butter?y in Fig. 2: it is hidden in its silk and leaf shelter during daylight hours, emerging to feed at dusk or night. Its false eyes are exposed when its shelter is torn open; and at that time, it thrusts the ?face? of its head out at the intru- der instead of retreating or turning away (or simply starting to repair its shelter). This behavior is shared with more than 100 species of ACG skipperbutter?ycaterpillarsandwith manyspeciesinotherfamilies.Again, the skipper butter?ypupainFig.1 spends 2 weeks hidden in a silked, rolled leaf and is visually exposed only when a diurnal predator opens that shelter. Then the pupa, which is ?rmly anchored at its base, twists on this anchor so as to project its ?face? out of the entrance at the forager. (ii) Also occur on (often large) caterpil- lars or pupae that live fully exposed, but with their false eye(s) often hid- den in folds of cuticle until explic- itly and ostentatiously displayed by the caterpillar in reaction to the approach or touch of a ?large? ob- ject. The false eyes in Figs. 3 B, H, I, K, and L are visible as false eyes only when the caterpillar expands and displays the crucial body part. (iii) Usually occur on caterpillars and pu- pae that are otherwise cryptically colored and patterned (rather than ostentatious); and these mimetic features are not visible at any signif- icant distance, even when the cater- pillar or pupa lives fully exposed. For example, the ground colors and patterns of the caterpillars and pu- pae in Figs. 3 and 4 are generally green, gray, brown, or black, rather than bright red, yellow, or blue. (iv) Are not of any one speci?c ?eye? shape or color but rather range from astonishingly detailed mimics of snake eyes and scales (e.g., Fig. 4H)tominimalsuggestionsofpaired approximate circles or dots in sur- rounding face-like patterns (e.g., Fig. 4 B, F, and G). Even when approximate, these patterns are suf- ?ciently eye-like and face-like to stimulate visual receptors/mental processes that vertebrate predators have evolved for rapidly recognizing what might be an eye, regardless of how imperfectly or fractionally seen (seerefs.5and15forelaboration).It ishardtobeconvincedthatthefalse eyes in Fig. 1 are not real, and we suspect even harder for a small bird when foraging (e.g., see ref. 2 for an extratropical example). (v) Are usually paired and evolution- arily derived from paired, more or less circular structures (e.g., pupal spiracles) or patterns. (On occasion, median circular patterns are the evolutionary precursors of one-eyed mimics, especially in caterpillars of Sphingidae and Notodontidae.) False eyes are not derived from real cat- erpillar ?eyes? (stemmata), which are tiny light sensors on the lower ?cheeks? of the head, or from the position of future real eyes inside Janzen et al. PNAS Early Edition | 3of7 Fig. 3. Representative ACG caterpillar false eyes and faces (see SI Appendix, Table S1 for names and voucher codes and SI Appendix, Figs. S3?S8 and ref. 24 for lateral and dorsal views of the same species of caterpillars). 4of7 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0912122107 Janzen et al. Fig. 4. Representative ACG pupa false eyes and faces (see SI Appendix, Table S1 for names and voucher codes and SI Appendix, Figs. S9?S14 and ref. 24 for lateral and dorsal views of the same species of pupae). Janzen et al. PNAS Early Edition | 5of7 the pupa. The external surfaces of the paired, pupal thoracic spiracles have frequently given rise to pupal false eyespots (e.g., all of the false eyes in Figs. 1 and 4 are evolution- arily modi?ed thoracic spiracles). However, we add that there may be selection to enhance almost any shape and color that can give the hint of eyes (e.g., refs. 5, 15). (vi) Are usually on the head end or the rear end of the caterpillar, and on the front end of the pupa. These are thepartsthatapredatorismostlikely toseewhenprobingthesiteofcater- pillar or pupa concealment and that resemble in position and shape the most dangerous part of a predator?s predator. Not emphasized in Fig. 3 (butseeFig.3AandF)isthefactthat these caterpillars often strike a sinu- ous pose with the body at the same time the head or front bearing false eyespots is thrust at the intruder. (vii) Are often combined with other col- ors and shapes that, when viewed from different directions, preserve or enhance the deception. This may include Escher-like illusions and transformations. For example, the same falseeyes andassociatedfacial patternsofthepupainFig.1givethe illusion of two different faces, de- pending on whether they are viewed from above or from the front. (viii) Are present in almost all ACG Lepidoptera families with large caterpillars and pupae (2?10 cm in length), and even in some fami- lies (e.g., Limacodidae, Crambi- dae, and Elachistidae) with quite small caterpillars (only 1?2cmin length). Although a 1?2-cm, gen- erally green to brown caterpillar inside a tangle of silk and leaves might seem impossibly small for a snake mimic, the frontal false eyes coupled with highly sinuous movements may well elicit a ?ight reaction by a small bird operating largelyonre?exesinclosequarters. (ix) Have independently evolved in nu- meroustaxonomiclineages.However, there are also species-rich clades within, for example, the Hesperiidae and the genus Xylophanes of the Sphingidae, in which the counterfeit eyes and faces of caterpillars and pu- pae (Figs. 1?4andSI Appendix, Figs. S1 and S2) apparently stem phylogenetically from a single evo- lutionary event instead of through convergence. (x) Are also encountered?although less frequently?on extratropical species of caterpillars (e.g., Pterourus,Pap- ilionidae; Xylophanes, Sphingidae). However, these caterpillars are often subjecttopredatorpressurebyinsect- eating migrant birds that spend major parts of their lives in the tropics (and often evolutionarily originated there) and therefore may extend the syn- drome envisioned here far outside of the tropics and into habitats that are less rich in predators on small birds than are many tropical ecosystems. (xi) May be overlooked by the casual observer owing to the plethora of additional caterpillar and pupa col- ors and patterns (and the many forms of crypsis) that the animals present in ?standard? lateral or dor- sal views [e.g., see SI Appendix for the lateral and dorsal views of the same caterpillars (SI Appendix, Figs. S3?S7)andpupae(SI Appendix, Figs. S8?S14) as in face and rear views in Figs. 3 and 4 and SI Ap- pendix, Figs. S1 and S2]. The great abundance and species rich- ness of caterpillars and pupae in tropical foliage suggest that the foraging insec- tivorous bird may encounter tens to hun- dreds of false-eyed individuals per day (more at low to medium elevations than at elevations above 1,500 m, which have fewer species of large caterpillars and leaf- rollers). There is no reason to postulate that the bird would learn about each spe- cies individually and mentally compare it with other predator-mimicking species, or compare its false eyes with those of any particular species of potential predator. Conclusion We postulate that all of these false-eyed species collectively constitute an enor- mous mimicry complex that is evolution- arily generated and sustained by the diverse actions and foraging traits of a large and multispeci?c array of avian predators that are innately programmed to instantly ?ee when in startlingly close proximity to the eye of another species, or to something that resembles such an eye. As stated at the outset, the bird that must learn to avoid an eye is not long for this world. In contrast to classical Batesian mimicry?in which the mimics are generally thought to be signi?- cantly rarer than the models?there are many hundreds of false-eyed cater- pillars and pupae for every vertebrate predator per hectare of tropical forest. This proportion is maintained by the ex- tremely high cost paid by the foraging bird that makes the mistake of pausing when encountering what might be an eye of one of its predators, coupled with the low price paid by passing up a poten- tial morsel. There have been arguments as to the existence of mimicry among caterpillars (see review in ref. 21). Our conclusion through the ongoing caterpillar survey of ACG is that essentially all tropical cater- pillars that live exposed, and many of those that do not, are visually mimetic of something?an inedible background or object (22), some other aposematic or mimetic caterpillar, a dangerous predator, or some combination of these. The multispeci?c diversity of caterpillar and pupal false eyes is evolutionarily gen- erated and maintained by the activities of a heterogeneous array of species of birds (and perhaps some small primates). Theserangefrom?xed-behavior(?stupid?) birds to ones that are ?smart? and plastic learners. Only a moderate number of in- dividuals and species of ?xedly (innately) dupable birds may be required to main- tain a large array of false eye and face patterns on many species of caterpillars and pupae. These species of birds may evolutionarily drive each of the eye-like and face-like patterns to be something more similar to an eye and/or face as they perceive it, without any reference to the false eyes and faces of other co- occurring caterpillar species. We suggest that each bird is responding to an eye-like or face-like stimulus, even though that stimulus is only an approximation of the real eye or face of any particular species of predator, or the false eye or face of any co- occurring species of caterpillar or pupa. The generally great advantage of false eyes and faces is not seriously diminished by the existence of some species of birds that can quickly determine that the mimetic caterpillar or pupa is edible (see ref. 6). Indeed, it can be postulated that selection may even work against exact resemblance among mimics because that could lead to a superabundance of one particular false eye and/or face pattern, thereby in- creasing the likelihood of a bird species or guild learning to associate that pattern with a harmless meal at the moment of encounter. We wish to emphasize that, in high- lighting the role of innate avoidance of threats by potential predators in this analysis and discussion of mimicry, we do not intend to diminish the one-to-one and one-on-one approaches inherent in many Batesian and M?llerian experimen- tal mimicry studies. Rather, we wish to broaden our understanding by recognizing that when the avoidance is innate, various assumptions, hypotheses, tests, and 6of7 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0912122107 Janzen et al. interpretations of mimicry may need to be modi?ed. Equally, we emphasize that what is a mimic in the eyes or mind of one predator may not be to another. There are models and mimics, and actions that are learned and innate, and they do not map perfectly on one another across the species and situations in which they occur. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS.Wethank theAreadeCon- servacion Guanacaste (ACG) team of parataxono- mists (10) for their support in ?nding and rearing the caterpillars and pupae discussed here; the ACG administration for saving the forest where these caterpillars and pupae live; and four anon- ymous reviewers for their comments on the man- uscript. This study was supported by US Nati- onal Science Foundation Grants BSR 9024770 and DEB 9306296, 9400829, 9705072, 0072730, and 0515699 and grants from Guanacaste Dry Forest Conservation Fund and ACG (D.H.J.). 1. Wallace AR (1881) The theory of descent. Book review. Nature 24:457?458. 2. Weismann A (1882) Studies in the Theory of Descent, trans Meldola R (Sampson Low, Marston, Searle & Rivington, London), Vol II, pp 401?729. 3. Blest AD (1957) The function of eyespot patterns in the Lepidoptera. Behaviour 11:209?256. 4. Greene HW, McDiarmid RW (2005) Wallace and Savage: Heroes, theories, and venomous snake mimicry. Ecology andEvolutionintheTropics:AHerpetologicalPerspective, eds Donnelly MA, Crother BI, Guyer CE, Wake MH, White ME(UnivChicago Press, Chicago), pp190?208. 5. Stevens M, Hardman CJ, Stubbins CL (2008) Conspi- cuousness, not eye mimicry, makes ?eyespots? effective antipredator signals. Behav Ecol 19:525?531. 6. Brower JVZ (1960) Experimental studies of mimicry. IV. The reactions of starlings to different proportions of models and mimics. Am Nat 94:271?282. 7. Shirota Y (1980) Protective function of eyespots in cater- pillars. Jpn J Entomol 48:1?5. 8. Smith SM (1975) Innate recognition of coral snake pattern by a possible avian predator. Science 187:759?760. 9. Janzen DH (1980) Two potential coral snake mimics in a tropical deciduous forest. Biotropica 12:77?78. 10. Janzen DH, et al. (2009) Integration of DNA barcoding into an ongoing inventory of complex tropical bio- diversity. Mol Ecol Resources 9(Suppl 1):1?26. 11. Janzen DH (1985) On ecological ?tting. Oikos 45: 308?310. 12. Pough FH (1988) Mimicry of vertebrates: Are the rules different? Am Nat 131(Suppl):S67?S102. 13. Janzen DH, Pond CM (1976) Food and feeding behavior of a captive Costa Rican least pigmy owl. Brenesia 9:71?80. 14. Moeller K, Hurwitz R (2006) US Patent 7134926. Device and method for dissuading predators through startle response instinct. 15. Stevens M, et al. (2007) Field experiments on the effectiveness of ?eyespots? as predator deterrents. Anim Behav 74:1215?1227. 16. Kodandaramaiah U, Vallin A, Wiklund C (2009) Fixed eyespot display in a butter?y thwarts attacking birds. Anim Behav 77:1415?1419. 17. Owen J, Gilbert FS (1989) On the abundance of hover?ies (Syrphidae). Oikos 55:183?193. 18. Howarth B, Edmunds M (2000) The phenology of Syrphidae (Diptera): Are they Batesian mimics of Hymenoptera? Biol J Linn Soc 71:437?457. 19. Swynnerton CFM (1926) An investigation into the defenses of butter?ies of the genus Charaxes. Proc Third Entomol Congress, Zurich (1925) 2:478?506. 20. Robbins RK (1981) The ?false head? hypothesis. Pre- dation and wing pattern variation of lycaenid butter- ?ies. Am Nat 118:770?775. 21. Berenbaum MR (1995) Aposematism and mimicry in caterpillars. J Lepid Soc 49:386?396. 22. Skelhorn J, Rowland HM, Speed MP, Ruxton GD (2010) Masquerade: Camou?age without crypsis. Science 327:51. 23. Burns JM (1996) Genitalia and the proper genus: Codatractus gets mysie and uvydixa?in a compact cyda group?as well as a hysterectomy, while Cephise gets part of Polythrix (Hesperiidae: Pyrginae). J Lepid Soc 50:173?216. 24. Miller JC, Janzen DH, Hallwachs W (2006) 100 Caterpillars (Harvard Univ Press, Cambridge, MA). Janzen et al. PNAS Early Edition | 7of7