g g D , U te, elic ial ve hy . W thi rato mp n si at tive ign nta e a ; H to visit tropica mid-nin ated a 140 ye remain is. Her butterfl data as Com mals, i especia in bees [8–10] ‘a beha close p hours’ from th [11,12] been o repeate range t at thei depart mates are generally conspecifics, but sometimes different ing to atures, warn- atism types, signals ch as sema- hieved tterns. apose- cts of mount ittle is nd its osting al stu- ion of ences ations lection etime, oured explanations have been narrowed to two major hypotheses: information sharing and/or anti-predator defence. The information-sharing hypothesis proposes that new roost mates, presumably related individuals, follow experienced on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from * Author for correspondence (sfinkbei@uci.edu).Received Acceptedthe Amazon, reported communal roosting in the l Heliconius passion-vine butterflies [3]. Since the eteenth century, this unusual behaviour has gener- great deal of scientific and popular interest. After ars of work on butterfly roosting, however, it still s unclear what the benefit of being a social butterfly e, we test the major hypotheses for why Heliconius ies roost communally, and present experimental sessing the adaptive function of this behaviour. munal roosting is observed in many types of ani- ncluding birds, bats and primates [4–7], and is lly widespread in insects, having been observed , wasps, beetles, dragonflies, butterflies and moths . In butterflies, communal roosting is described as vior in which individuals aggregate quiescently in roximity to each other at a site for more than a few ([9] p. 90). This behaviour is known primarily e heliconiines, acraeines, ithomiines and danaiines . Many species of Heliconius in particular have bserved to form communal roosts in which adults dly gather in a particular location in their home o roost for the night (figure 1a). Butterflies arrive r roost sites as early as 3 h before sunset, and from roosts within the first 2 h after sunrise. Roost coloured wing patterns and their unpalatability ow cyanogenic glycosides [18]. Because of these fe Heliconius butterflies serve as a textbook example of ing signalling—also known as aposematism. Aposem is a major theme in the evolution of animal pheno where its principal function is to provide warning associated with unprofitability to predators, su toxicity, unpalatability or capture costs [19–21]. Apo tism is widespread in invertebrates and is often ac through visual signalling via conspicuous colour pa Collective aposematism is a phenomenon in which matic prey form aggregations to enhance the effe warning signals [22,23]. Despite the substantial a that is known about Heliconius natural history, l known about their communal roosting behaviour a possible relationship to collective aposematism. There is a broad literature on Heliconius ro [12,13,24–29]; however, relatively few experiment dies have been performed to address the funct this behaviour. Although the adaptive consequ of roosting remain unclear, it is unlikely that aggreg are involved with thermoregulation [10], kin se [13] or mating (females usually mate once in their lif within hours or days after eclosion) [30,31]. The favroosting in butterflies [1,2]. Shortly thereafter W. H. Edwards, the entomologist who inspired Bates and Wallace long lifespan owing to pollen consumption [16,17]. Heliconius butterflies are well known for their brightlyThe benefit of bein communal roostin Susan D. Finkbeiner1,*, Adriana 1Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology 2Smithsonian Tropical Research Institu Aposematic passion-vine butterflies from the genusH behaviour has been hypothesized to be benefic predator defence. To better understand the adapti hypotheses in field studies. The information-sharing haviour of butterflies departing from natural roosts another to resources, thus providing no support for was tested using avian-indiscriminable Heliconius e sites. A significantly higher number of predation atte tions ofmodels. This relationship between aggregatio butterflies enjoy the benefits of both overall decreased munal roosts probably deter predators through collec unpalatable prey communicate amore effective repel s that predation by birds is a key selective pressure mai Keywords: communal roosting; collectiv predation 1. INTRODUCTION In 1867, the naturalist J. A. Allen first described the spectacular aggregations of migratory monarchs, and was thus the first to report the phenomenon of communal27 January 2012 29 February 2012 2769a social butterfly: deters predation . Briscoe1 and Robert D. Reed1,2 niversity of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA Panama City 0843-03092, Panama onius form communal roosts on a nightly basis. This in terms of information sharing and/or anti- value of communal roosting, we tested these two pothesis was addressed by examining following be- e found no evidence of roost mates following one s hypothesis. The anti-predator defence hypothesis models placed singly and in aggregations at field ts were observed on solitary models versus aggrega- ze and attack rate suggests that communally roosting tack frequency as well as a prey dilution effect. Com- aposematism inwhich aggregations of conspicuous, al to predators. On the basis of our results, we propose ining Heliconius communal roosting behaviour. posematism; dilution effect; aggregation; eliconius species—often Mu¨llerian co-mimics—roost together [13]. The evolution of communal roosting behaviour inHeliconius is believed to be facilitated by unpalatability, slow repro- ductive rate [14], limited learned home range [12,15] and Proc. R. Soc. B (2012) 279, 2769–2776 doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.0203 Published online 21 March 2012This journal is q 2012 The Royal Society (( o. ( 2770 S. D. Finkbeiner et al. Butterfly communal roosting on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from (c) Figure 1. (a) Natural and (b) artificial roosts of Heliconius erat(a)members from the roost to food sources [27,32]. This form of information sharing is a common behaviour in other communal animals such as birds [33,34]. Conversely, the anti-predator hypothesis suggests predators avoid Heliconius aggregations as a result of collective aposematism or predator confusion [12,13,35]. In other aposematic insects, gregarious behaviour contributes to collective enhancement of warning signalling, resulting inmore effec- tive predator deterrence [22,36–38]. Another potential anti-predator mechanism is the prey dilution effect, often known as ‘safety in numbers’, which posits that the prob- ability of a single individual being attacked in a group is lower with increasing density [39–42]. In field studies in Panama and Costa Rica, we tested these hypotheses to determine why Heliconius passion- vine butterflies assemble in communal roosts. To test whether Heliconius butterflies rely on roosts as infor- mation-sharing centres, we examined following behaviour by butterflies during departures from natural aggregations. The anti-predator defence hypothesis was tested using avian-indiscriminable artificial butterfly models placed singly and in aggregations in the forest. Following the pre- dation study, we investigated whether naturally occurring roost sizes correspond with optimal roost sizes inferred from experimental data. 2. MATERIAL AND METHODS (a) Field sites All data collection was completed in Costa Rica and Panama. Field sites for this work were chosen based on the abundance mark imprint on wax wing (indicated by arrow). (d) Dorsal view Proc. R. Soc. B (2012)b) d) c) Beak pinch at end of model abdomen, and triangular beakand accessibility of Heliconius butterflies and communal roosts. The Panama sites were part of the Smithsonian Tro- pical Research Institute; we used areas in Gamboa and Soberanı´a National Park along Pipeline Road. Data were col- lected in Panama from June through September of 2010 and 2011 during the rainy season. The 2010 visit resulted in natural roost data collected from Heliconius erato, and the 2011 visit resulted in the predation experiment data. In Costa Rica, we worked at the Organization for Tropical Studies’ La Selva Tropical Biological Station in Sarapiquı´. La Selva was visited in April and May of 2011, during the end of the dry season into the beginning of the rainy season. This site was used for collecting natural roost data from Heliconius sara and model predation experiments. (b) Natural roost observations To assess the information-sharing hypothesis, which predicts that butterflies use roosts to learn the locations of foraging sites from other roost mates, we observed following behaviour of H. erato and H. sara individuals departing natural roosting aggregations. Following was confirmed only if a butterfly was observed departing the roost with another roost mate to sub- sequently arrive at a flowering plant with that same roost mate. The roosts were within 30 m of the first visited flowering plant, and it was feasible to follow butterflies to these plants. We began all observations approximately 30 min before sun- rise, before butterflies left to forage, and stayed until only one individual remained. All H. erato roost members were given unique identification numbers using a Sharpie marker, and were sexed and age determined based on wing wear [43]. Butterflies were captured and marked after departing of abdominal beak pinch. single butterflies were used for the first predation experiment. The models were placed in 80 different forest sites, each con- taining four roosts and four single individuals. All 80 sites were at least 250 m apart to control for the home range ter- ritories of primary Heliconius avian predators. Predator home range sizes vary between 100 and 250 m, and have been determined by other researchers through radio tracking (flycatchers [54]), harmonic distance method and core area use (tanagers [55]), minimum convex polygon modelling (flycatchers [54]) and observation (tanagers [56]; jacamars [57]; C. E. G. Pinheiro 2011, personal communication; L. E. Gilbert 2011, personal communication). Tree Tanglefoot 0 80(b) 60 40 pe r c en t r ef le ct an ce 20 0 80 300 400 natural wing black (forewing) A A B A B natural wing black (hindwing) B natural wing pink (light) A natural wing pink (dark) B model black model pink 500 wavelength (nm) 600 700 (c) 60 40 pe r c en t r ef le ct an ce 20 0 Figure 2. Reflectance spectra of natural and artificial butter- fly wings used in discriminability calculations. Shown are the mean values and standard errors (each n ¼ 12). Graphs include a ventral image of Heliconius erato petiverana with circled regions indicating where wing reflectance measure- ments were taken. (a) Yellow reflectance spectra. (b) Pink reflectance spectra, comparisons made using lightest A and darkest B pink regions on the wing. (c) Black reflectance spectra, comparisons made using both forewing A and hindwing B regions. Butterfly communal roosting S. D. Finkbeiner et al. 2771 on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from their roosts to prevent them from associating the roost with danger [44]. Average roost sizes were determined by recording the number of individuals in each roost nightly. (c) Models We used artificial butterfly models to test the hypothesis that Heliconius roosts provide an anti-predatory benefit. Wing images were designed in Adobe ILLUSTRATOR using high- resolution scans of ventral Heliconius erato petiverana wings as a reference. Model butterfly wings were printed on What- man filter paper, which produces reflectance spectra close in brightness to actual wings, using an Epson Stylus Pro 4880 printer with UltraChrome K3 ink. A 3-hydroxy-DL- kynurenine (3-OHK) pigment solution of 1.0 mg 3-OHK dissolved in 100 ml of methanol was applied to the yellow bands on the hindwing to provide accurate UV reflectance, because printed yellows do not accurately mimic Heliconius yellow in the shape of their reflectance spectra. 3-OHK is the same yellow wing pigment as found in the wings of the butterflies themselves [45,46]. Portions of the artificial wings were dipped in clear wax to allow imprints of beak and bite marks, then Krylon matte finishing spray was applied lightly (before spectra measurements were taken) to coat the 3-OHK with a waterproofing element. Model abdomens were made of Newplast Plasticine. Birds, in particular jacamars, flycatchers and tanagers, are major Heliconius predators [27,36,47–50]. Therefore, artifi- cial butterfly models were designed and assessed using tetrachromatic bird colour-vision models in order to ensure that avian predators would find colour stimuli presented by the models indiscriminable from actual butterflies. Reflec- tance spectra of yellow, pink and black from the models and the ventral surface of natural H. erato petiverana wings were measured using an Ocean Optics USB2000 fibre optic spectrometer. A deuterium–halogen tungsten lamp (DH- 2000, Ocean Optics) was used as a standardized light source, and measurements were taken using a bifurcating fibre cable (R400-7-UV–vis, Ocean Optics). The axis of the illuminating and detecting fibre was at an elevation of 458 to the plane of the wing and pointed left with respect to the body axis for every measurement. A white spectralon standard (WS-1-SL, Labsphere) was used to calibrate the spectrometer. Spectra measurements from the fibre optic spectrometer were processed using MATLAB software (see Briscoe et al. [46]). The quantum catches for stimuli [51] were calculated, and discriminability between artificial models and natural wing reflectance spectra was determined using tetrachromatic bird-vision models from Vorobyev & Osorio [52]. The comparisons were made using the blue tit (Parus caeruleus) and chicken (Gallus gallus) cone sensi- tivities, which represent the UV- and violet-type avian visual systems, respectively. Low light intensity and open habitat irradiance spectra were used [53]. All spectral com- parisons represented by an average of wing measurements (n ¼ 12) fell below the threshold of one just noticeable differ- ence (figure 2 and table 1); therefore, the reflectance spectra of models and actual butterfly wings were inferred to be indiscriminable to birds. (d) Predation experiments Butterfly models were tied to branches with thread in appro- priate roosting habitats and in natural roosting postures [10,13] (figure 1b). At our Costa Rica field site, a total of 320 aggregations containing five butterflies each and 320Proc. R. Soc. B (2012)80(a) 60 40 pe r c en t r ef le ct an ce 20 A natural wing yellow (A) model yellow site included as a random effect. The post hoc tests for the lig allu tice HV mo 0.7 0.8 2772 S. D. Finkbeiner et al. Butterfly communal roosting on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from was applied to the base of plant stems containing artificial butterflies to avoid removal or attack of the models by ants and other small arthropods, and it was also effective in prevent- ing small vertebrates such as lizards from reaching the butterfly models [58]. The models were left at their sites for a total of 96 h (4 days), and each model was examined daily for predation evidence and replaced if attacked. None of the 80 sites were used twice in the study because predator forgetting time varies between bird species, and is affected by prey con- spicuousness and distastefulness [59–61], both difficult to measure for this project. A butterfly was considered attacked if damage to the abdomen and wings appeared in the form of beak marks and/or large indentations in the abdomen (figure 1c,d; see also [62,63]). Small chew-like marks, prob- ably from mandibular insects such as grasshoppers, were not considered in the data analyses. If a model was attacked twice on two separate days, then it was counted only as a single attack. If multiple butterflies in a roost were attacked, this was also counted as a single attack (i.e. each roost was treated as a unit), because in nature when a roost is disturbed most or all individuals depart from the roosting site [13,29] (S. D. Finkbeiner 2010, personal observation), thereby reducing the probability of further predation attempts on individual roost mates. Predation differences were analysed using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test with continuity correction, using each site as a sample. To assess the per capita attack risk of individual butterflies, single (focal) individuals were randomly selected from each roost to compare attack rates under the conservative assump- tion that one attack leads to the dispersal of the other roosting butterflies. For roosts of one, this single individual was the focal individual. The binomial response of attack (yes, no) was modelled as dependent upon roost size using generalized linear models. In these analyses, site was included as a random effect to account for potential non-independence among replicate roosts within sites. As a control for our models, we compared the attack rate on models with real wings (and Plasticine abdomens) to the attack rate on models with artificial wings, using five forest sites sep- Table 1. Results from discriminability calculations using low made using the blue tit (Parus caeruleus) and chicken (Gallus g Spectra comparisons fell below the threshold of one just no ventral; L, light; D, dark. JND comparisons FV-L pink versus model pink FV-D pink versus model pink P. caeruleus 0.597 0.374 G. gallus 0.556 0.424arated 250 m apart, each with four models with real wings and four models with artificial wings. They were left for 96 h and checked daily for attacks. We found no difference in attacks between real-wingmodels and artificial-wingmodels (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: W¼ 249.5, p ¼ 0.836, n ¼ 20). The second predation study, conducted in Panama, inves- tigated the association between roost size and predation frequency. This was tested using the same artificial butterfly models described earlier. One hundred forest sites were chosen, approximately 250 m apart, and each of these sites contained two roosts of 2, two roosts of 5 and two roosts of 10 butterflies. This totalled 600 artificial roosts: 200 roosts Proc. R. Soc. B (2012)significance of pairwise comparisons were made using a Tukey test. We determined at what time of day roosts are most sus- ceptible to attack to gain further information about the roost predators. This study was conducted with 100 artificial roosts in Panama: 52 roosts of two placed randomly along two trails in Soberanı´a National Park, with the remaining 48 roosts already being observed for predation data (16 more roosts of 2, 5 and 10 each). The artificial roosts were checked for attacks every 3 h from 06.00 (15 min prior to sunrise) to 18.00 (just before sunset) for 9 days. 3. RESULTS (a) Observations of roost departure following To assess whether Heliconius butterflies rely on communal roosts as information-sharing centres, we observed fol- lowing behaviour during morning roost departures. Of 256 H. erato departures by at least 66 unique individuals from nine different roosts, only one instance of following from the roost to a flowering plant was observed (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: W ¼ 256, p , 0.0001, n ¼ 256, based on the null hypothesis that there is no differ- ence between the number of butterflies that do and do not follow). Additionally, out of 74 H. sara butterfly departures from at least 25 unique individuals and threefor each treatment. Roosts were removed after 4 days, each of the 100 sites was used only once and attacked models were replaced when necessary. If a roost was attacked more than once or if more than one butterfly in the roost was attacked, then it was counted only as one attack; thus each roost was considered as a single unit in the analysis. These predation data were analysed using a Kruskal–Wallis mul- tiple comparison test with a post hoc Bonferroni correction, using each site as a sample. Individual per capita attack risk was determined by comparing attack rates between randomly selected single (focal) individuals in each roost treatment, and the binomial response of attack was modelled as depen- dent upon roost size using generalized linear models, with ht intensity and open habitat irradiance. Comparisons were s) cone sensitivities, and are based on mean values (n ¼ 12). able difference (JND). FV, forewing ventral; HV, hindwing yellow versus del yellow FV black versus model black HV black versus model black 36 0.336 0.440 58 0.315 0.533different roosts, we observed no incidence of follow- ing. When butterflies departed roosts in the morning occasionally more than one individual would leave at the same time, but they were never observed to follow one another. Most of the time the butterflies left individually, even when there was a disturbance event. Lantana and Psychotria plants, considered to be key Heliconius resources, were common at our study sites, and it is important to note that spatial distribution and density of nectar and pollen sources may influence whether follow- ing happens. We observed that many roosting butterflies shared the same flower resources and followed each other (b) Observations on roost size and proximity (c) Effect of roost size on predation frequency ld roos tes ds s. Butterfly communal roosting S. D. Finkbeiner et al. 2773 on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from To determine whether communal roosts provide an anti-predatory benefit, we first tested whether there is a difference in predation between single butterfly models or models placed in aggregations of five. We observed a very strong difference in attack frequencies between roost- ing and solitary butterflies. Of 320 artificial H. eratoThe average H. erato roost size was observed to be 4.3 individuals (s.d. ¼ 1.6, n ¼ 233 observations across nine natural roosts) from roosts in Gamboa, Panama. It was common to find multiple roosts within 15 m of each other, some as close as 3 m apart, and in line-of-sight from one another in a given part of the home range. We observed this in both H. erato and H. sara. When but- terflies of H. erato were exercising pre-roosting behaviour, the butterflies often interacted with one another before convening at their preferred roosts.between flowering plants, as previously described by Waller & Gilbert [27]. As well, on multiple occasions, we observed a new recruit following an established roost member to the aggregation, suggesting following behaviour may play a role in roost recruitment. Table 2. Data representing attacks on butterfly models at fie size. Roost attack risk was calculated by dividing attacked Individual attack risk was determined by comparing attack ra roost treatment, based on the assumption that one attack lea values between pairwise comparisons are indicated by asterisk aggregation size total observations roosts attacked Costa Rica single individuals 320 68g***roost of five 320 25 Panama roost of two 200 21g* roost of five 200 8 roost of 10 200 *f24 *p, 0.05. **p, 0.001. ***p, 0.0005.roosts, only 25 were attacked compared with 68 attacks on 320 single individuals (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: W ¼ 4141.5, p ¼ 0.000262, n ¼ 80 sites; table 2). Indi- vidual attack risk also differed significantly between roosts of 1 (single butterflies) and roosts of 5 (F1,559 ¼ 36.85, p , 0.0001, n ¼ 640 roosts total), with 21.3 per cent of focal individuals being attacked in roosts of 1 but only 3.4 per cent of focal individuals being attacked in roosts of 5 (table 2). Considering the difference we found in predation between single butterflies and roosts, we decided to inves- tigate whether the predator deterrence effect was similar across roosts of different sizes (table 2). We found signifi- cantly higher predation on roosts of 2 and 10 than on roosts of 5 (Kruskal–Wallis test: x2¼ 8.7356, p ¼ 0.0127, n ¼ 100 sites). The post hoc Bonferroni correction showed that roosts of 10 were attacked more than roosts of 5 (p ¼ 0.016), and roosts of 2 were attacked more than roosts of 5 (p ¼ 0.028). There was no significant Proc. R. Soc. B (2012)difference in predation frequency between roosts of 2 and 10 (p ¼ 1.000). Individual attack rate also differed significantly between the three roost sizes (F2,498 ¼ 5.51, p ¼ 0.0043, n ¼ 600 roosts total), with attack rates on focal individuals in roosts of 2, 5 and 10 being 9.5 per cent, 1.5 per cent and 4.5 per cent, respectively (table 2). Adjusting significance levels to account for mul- tiple comparisons, Tukey tests showed that a single butterfly in a roost of 2 has a higher attack risk than a single butterfly in a roost of 5 (p ¼ 0.00064), but there is no difference in individual attack risk between roosts of 5 and 10 butterflies (p ¼ 0.22), or between roosts of 2 and 10 butterflies (p ¼ 0.13). (d) Timing and nature of predation We sought to determine at which time of day roosts are most susceptible to attack. We found that all attacks occurred during the morning hours, as previously observed by Mallet [13]. Ten out of 12 attacks, from 100 different artificial roosts, occurred between the hours of 06.00 and 09.00, and two attacks occurred between the hours of 09.00 and 12.00. Triangular beak marks observed in the model abdomens supports pre- vious findings [48–50] that birds are the primary predators of Heliconius. This is further supported because all attacks on models occurred between the hours ofsites in Costa Rica and Panama, categorized by aggregation ts by the overall number of roosts used in that treatment. between randomly selected single (focal) individuals in each to the dispersal of the other roosting butterflies. Probability attack risk (per roost, %) attack risk (per individual, %) 21.3 21.3g***7.8 3.4 10.5 9.5g**4.0 1.5 12.0 4.506.00 and 12.00 when birds are most active [56,64]. 4. DISCUSSION In this study, we assessed the two major hypotheses for explaining why Heliconius butterflies participate in com- munal roosting behaviour. We found no support for the information-sharing hypothesis, because there was little evidence of roost mates following each other to resources upon departure from roosts. These findings are in agree- ment with Mallet [13], who observed a similar lack of following and even a predictable tendency for roost mates to visit different flowers. In contrast, we found very strong support for the anti- predator defence hypothesis. Our field experiments in Costa Rica using avian-indiscriminable butterfly models showed predation attempts on singly placed models were nearly three times higher than predation attempts on roosts of five models, and the predation risk for a butterflies are aware of other roosts in their home range, yet still choose to join smaller aggregations, despite 2774 S. D. Finkbeiner et al. Butterfly communal roosting on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from single butterfly is over six times the per capita preda- tion risk for an individual butterfly in a roost of 5 (table 2). A second field experiment in Panama showed the same trend, with attack rates more than twice as high on roosts of 2 versus 5, and individual risk over six times higher in a roost of 2 than a roost of 5 (table 2). Surpris- ingly, however, the Panama experiment also showed that attacks on roosts of 10 were three times as high as on roosts of 5 (table 2), thus suggesting the predator deter- rence effect may be weak or non-existent in large aggregations. In the Panama experiment, the greatest indi- vidual fitness benefit was seen in roosts of 5, with an individual predation risk of 1.5 per cent; however, individ- uals in roosts of 10 benefited only slightly less (not significant) than those in roosts of 5 (individual risk of 4.5% in roosts of 10). Therefore, even though roosts of 10 did not enjoy a significantly decreased predation rate compared with roosts of 2 or 5, a simple prey dilution effect [39] would still favour large roost sizes. Our field studies suggest that the most beneficial mini- mum roost size, with respect to group advantage, may be around 5 individuals. This is because our experimental aggregations of five models experienced the lowest overall attack rates and also offered the lowest per capita attack risk for individuals. Interestingly, this experimentally determined minimum roost size corresponds closely to naturally observed H. erato roost sizes (4.3). This corre- spondence implies that predator deterrence, coupled with a prey dilution effect, could help explain roost sizes observed in natural populations. An optimal or minimum roost size may be important for predator deterrence when butterfly densities are too low for assembling in larger aggregations; however, roost sizes are probably influenced by foraging and resource availability as well. Because medium-sized roosts provide an anti-predatory benefit through collective aposematism, it is unclear why larger aggregations appear to lose their ability to deter pre- dators. It is possible that solitary individuals or very small roosts are too small to communicate an effective warning signal, whereas very large roosts may be conspicuous enough to attract naı¨ve predators. In support of this idea, Salcedo [29] noted that themost frequent predator disturb- ances on H. sara roosts occurred on the largest aggregation studied (10–16 individuals), suggesting that oversized aggregations of Heliconius may increase the frequency of predator attacks. Although there is much evidence that predator wariness and aggregation-induced phobias increase with the size of aposematic prey aggregations [61], greater aggregation distinctiveness increases detect- ability costs and, in some cases, larger aggregation sizes of defended animals result in higher predation [23,65]. There may be other costs to forming larger groups such that small movements made by other butterflies could pro- duce incidental disturbances or ‘false alarms’, causing unexpected or premature roost departures, but this would not explain higher predation on larger roosts from our experimental data. We propose that it is no coincidence that multiple roosts are found in the same location in a home range and often in line-of-sight from one another. We have observed this in H. erato, H. sara, H. melpomene and H. charithonia, with some roosts neighbouring the roosts of heterospecifics (in Costa Rica). Others have observed this Heliconius behaviour of preferentially forming smallerProc. R. Soc. B (2012)tion sizes. This correspondence suggests that predation may be the key selective pressure maintaining communal roosting in Heliconius, thus providing insight into the types of ecological pressures that contribute to the evolution of social behaviour in historically solitary animals. We thank Kailen Mooney for aid in project design and statistical analyses; Jim Mallet for advice and comments; Christian Salcedo and Daniel Osorio for feedback on the manuscript; Nancy Burley, Owen McMillan and Larry Gilbert for advice; Jasmine Velez, Talia Gustafson, Jimena Golcher and Maranatha Kellinger for aid in field observations and assisting in the preparation of butterfly models; Johannes Spaethe, Chris Jiggins and Patricio Salazar for tips on butterfly models; Dave Krueger and UCI ImageWorks for aid in designing and printing models; the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute (STRI) and Organization for Tropical Studies (OTS) for use of field sites; La Autoridad Nacional del Ambiente (ANAM, Panama) and El Ministerio del Ambiente, Energı´a, y Telecomunicaciones (MINAET, Costa Rica) for research permit approval; and our funding sources: the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institue, the Organization for Tropical Studies, the U.S. Department of Education GAANN, the National Geographic Society; this material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF) Graduate Research Fellowship under award no. DGE- 0808392 to S.D.F. and NSF grant no. IOS-1025106 to A.D.B. and R.D.R. REFERENCES 1 Scudder, S. H. & Allen, J. A. 1869 A preliminary list of the butterflies of Iowa. Trans. Chicago Acad. Sci. 1, 326–337. 2 Brower, L. P. 1995 Understanding and misunderstanding the migration of the monarch butterfly (Nymphalidae) in North America: 1857–1995. J. Lepid. Soc. 49, 304–385.plenty of substrate (i.e. dry branches and twigs) to sustain much larger aggregations. On the basis of the assumption that individual fitness should increase with larger aggrega- tions owing to a reduced per capita predation risk, again it is unclear why local butterflies do not simply choose to form very large roosts. There is the possibility that an interaction among roosts is introduced, so that if individ- uals in one group are attacked the predator is inhibited from attacking individuals in other groups [42]. This repeated warning display could therefore facilitate rapid learning in naı¨ve birds whose feeding areas may include multiple Heliconius roosting sites [67,68]. It is also poss- ible that very large roosts could attract high enough levels of predation to cancel out the prey dilution benefit, although experimental work is required to test this idea. Here, we show that Heliconius butterflies do not rely on communal roosts as information-sharing centres. Instead, our field studies indicate that this behaviour confers an anti-predatory benefit at both the individual level (prey dilution) and group level (collective aposematism), and attack risks vary between individuals in different aggrega-aggregations as well [13,66] (C. Boggs 2011, personal communication). Salcedo [29] proposes these may be early-stage aggregations made up of individuals who have not yet located a larger roost. In contrast, however, our field observations of pre-roosting interactions between butterflies from different roosts suggest that the Butterfly communal roosting S. D. Finkbeiner et al. 2775 on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from 3 Edwards, W. H. 1881 On certain habits of Heliconia char- itonia Linn., a species of butterfly found in Florida. Papilio 1, 209–215. 4 Ward, P. 1965 Feeding ecology of the black-faced dioch Quellea quellea in Nigeria. Ibis 107, 173–214. (doi:10. 1111/j.1474-919X.1965.tb07296.x) 5 Soini, P. 1987Ecology of the saddle-back tamarin Saguinus fuscicollis-illigeri on the Rio Pacaya, northeastern Peru.Folia Primatol. 49, 11–32. (doi:10.1159/000156305) 6 Lewis, S. E. 1995 Roost fidelity of bats: a review. J. Mammal. 76, 481–496. (doi:10.2307/1382357) 7 Merkel, F. R. & Mosbech, A. 2008 Diurnal and noctur- nal feeding strategies in common eiders. Waterbirds 31, 580–586. (doi:10.1675/1524-4695-31.4.580) 8 Pearson, D. L. & Anderson, J. J. 1985 Perching heights and nocturnal communal roosts of some tiger beetles (Coleoptera: Cicindelidae) in southeastern Peru. Biotropica 17, 126–129. (doi:10.2307/2388504) 9 DeVries, P. J., Schull, J. & Greig, N. 1987 Synchro- nous nocturnal activity and gregarious roosting in the neotropical skipper butterfly Celaenorrhinus fritzgaertneri (Lepidoptera: Hesperiidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 89, 89–103. (doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.1987.tb01345.x) 10 Salcedo, C. 2010 Environmental elements involved in communal roosting in Heliconius butterflies (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). Environ. Entom. 39, 907–911. (doi:10. 1603/EN09340) 11 Benson, W. W. & Emmel, T. C. 1973 Demography of gregariously roosting populations of the nymphaline butterfly Marpesia berania in Costa Rica. Ecology 54, 326–335. (doi:10.2307/1934340) 12 Turner, J. R. G. 1975 Communal roosting in relation to warning colour in two heliconiine butterflies (Nymphali- dae). J. Lepid. Soc. 29, 221–226. 13 Mallet, J. 1986 Gregarious roosting and home range in Heliconius butterflies. Natl. Geogr. Res. 2, 198–215. 14 Erlich, P. R. & Gilbert, L. E. 1973 Population structure and dynamics of the tropical butterfly Heliconius ethilla. Biotropica 5, 69–82. (doi:10.2307/2989656) 15 Mallet, J. 1986 Dispersal and gene flow in a butterfly with home range behavior: Heliconius erato (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). Oecologia 68, 210–217. (doi:10.1007/ BF00384789) 16 Gilbert, L. E. 1972 Pollen feeding and reproductive biology of Heliconius butterflies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 69, 1403–1407. (doi:10.1073/pnas.69.6.1403) 17 Boggs, C. L., Smiley, J. T. & Gilbert, L. E. 1981 Patterns of pollen exploitation by Heliconius butterflies. Oecologia 48, 284–289. (doi:10.1007/BF00347978) 18 Engler-Chaouat, H. S. & Gilbert, L. E. 2007 De novo synthesis versus sequestration: negatively correlated metabolic traits and the evolution of host plant specializ- ation in cyanogenic butterflies. J. Chem. Ecol. 33, 25–42. (doi:10.1007/s10886-006-9207-8) 19 Cott, H. B. 1957 Adaptive coloration in animals. London, UK: Methuen. 20 Guilford, T. 1990 The evolution of aposematism. In Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators (eds D. L. Evans & J. O. Schmidt), pp. 23–61. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. 21 Santos, J. C., Coloma, L. A. & Cannatella, D. C. 2003 Multiple, recurring origins of aposematism and diet specialization in poison frogs. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 12 792–12 797. (doi:10.1073/pnas.2133521100) 22 Gamberale, G. & Tullberg, B. S. 1998 Aposematism and gregariousness: the combined effect of group size and coloration on signal repellence. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 889–894. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0374) 23 Riipi, M., Alatalo, R. V., Lindstro¨m, L. & Mappes, J. 2001 Multiple benefits of gregariousnes cover detectability costsProc. R. Soc. B (2012)in aposematic aggregations. Nature 413, 512–514. (doi:10.1038/35097061) 24 Jones, F. M. 1930 The sleeping heliconias of Florida. Nat. Hist. 30, 635–644. 25 Young, A. M. & Thomason, J. H. 1975 Notes on com- munal roosting of Heliconius charitonius (Nymphalidae) in Costa Rica. J. Lepid. Soc. 29, 243–255. 26 Mallet, J. L. B. 1980 A laboratory study of gregarious roosting in the butterfly Heliconius melpomene. MSc thesis, University of Newcastle-upon-Tyne, Newcastle- upon-Tyne, UK. 27 Waller, D. A. & Gilbert, L. E. 1982 Roost recruitment and resource utilization: observations on a Heliconius charitonia L. roost in Mexico. J. Lepid. Soc. 36, 178–184. 28 Mallet, J. & Gilbert, L. E. 1995 Why are there so many mimicry rings? Correlations between habitat, behaviour and mimicry in Heliconius butterflies. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 55, 159–180. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1995.tb01057.x) 29 Salcedo, C. 2011 Evidence of predation and disturbance events at Heliconius (Insecta: Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae) nocturnal aggregations in Panama and Costa Rica. J. Nat. Hist. 45, 1715–1721. (doi:10.1080/00222933. 2011.559692) 30 Gilbert, L. E. 1976 Postmating female odor in Heliconius butterflies: male-contributed anti-aphrodisiac. Science 193, 419–420. (doi:10.1126/science.935877) 31 Schulz, S., Estrada, C., Yildizhan, S., Boppre´, M. & Gilbert, L. E. 2008 An antiaphrodisiac in Heliconius melpomene butterflies. J. Chem. Ecol. 34, 82–93. (doi:10.1007/s10886-007-9393-z) 32 Gilbert, L. E. 1975 Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants. In Coevolution of animals and plants (eds L. E. Gilbert & P. R. Raven), pp. 210–240. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. 33 Ward, P. & Zahavi, A. 1973 The importance of certain assemblages of birds as ‘information centers’ for food finding. Ibis 115, 517–534. (doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X. 1973.tb01990.x) 34 Dall, S. R. X. 2002 Can information sharing explain recruitment to food from communal roosts? Behav. Ecol. 13, 42–51. (doi:10.1093/beheco/13.1.42) 35 Gillett, S. D., Hogarth, P. J. & Noble, F. E. 1979 The response of predators to varying densities of Gregaria locust nymphs. Anim. Behav. 27, 592–596. (doi:10. 1016/0003-3472(79)90195-7) 36 Benson, W. W. 1971 Evidence for the evolution of unpalatability through kin selection in the Heliconiinae (Lepidoptera). Am. Nat. 105, 213–226. (doi:10.1086/ 282719) 37 Edmunds, M. 1974 Defence in animals: a survey of anti- predator defences. Harlow, UK: Longman. 38 Vulinek, K. 1990 Collective security: aggregation by insects as a defense. In Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators (eds D. L. Evans & J. O. Schmidt), pp. 251–288. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. 39 Bertram, B. C. R. 1978 Living in groups: predators and prey. In Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach (eds J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies), pp. 64–96. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. 40 Hamilton, W. D. 1971 Geometry for the selfish herd. J. Theor. Biol. 31, 295–311. (doi:10.1016/0022-5193 (71)90189-5) 41 Turner, G. F. & Pitcher, T. J. 1986 Attack abatement: a model for group protection by combined avoidance and dilution. Am. Nat. 128, 228–240. (doi:10.1086/284556) 42 Sille´n-Tullberg, B. & Leimar, O. 1988 The evolution of gregariousness in distasteful insects as a defense against predators. Am. Nat. 132, 723–734. (doi:10. 1086/284884) 43 Karlsson, B. 1987 Variation in egg weight, oviposition rate and reproductive reserves with female age in a natu- ral population of the speckled wood butterfly, Pararge aegeria. Ecol. Entomol. 12, 473–476. (doi:10.1111/j. 1365-2311.1987.tb01029.x) 44 Mallet, J., Longino, J. T., Murawski, D., Murawski, A. & Simpson de Gamboa, A. 1987 Handling effects in Helico- nius: where do all the butterflies go? J. Anim. Ecol. 56, 377–386. (doi:10.2307/5054) 45 Reed, R. D., McMillan, W. O. & Nagy, L. M. 2008 Gene expression underlying adaptive variation in Heliconius wing patterns: non-modular regulation of overlapping cinnabar and vermilion prepatterns. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 37–45. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1115) 46 Briscoe, A. D., Bybee, S. M., Bernard, G. D., Yuan, F., Front. Biol. China 2, 345–350. (doi:10.1007/s11515- 007-0051-1) 55 Samuel, M. D., Pierce, D. J. & Garton, E. O. 1985 Iden- tifying areas of concentrated use within home range. J. Anim. Ecol. 54, 711–719. (doi:10.2307/4373) 56 Buskirk, W. H., Powell, G. V. N., Wittenberger, J. F., Buskirk, R. E. & Powell, T. U. 1972 Interspecific bird flocks in tropical highland Panama. The Auk 89, 612–624. 57 Pinheiro, C. E. G., Bagno, M. A. & Branda˜o, R. A. 2003 Diet and foraging behavior of the rufous-tailed jacamar (Galbula ruficauda, Galbulidae) in central Brazil. Ararajuba 11, 241–243. 58 Dial, R. & Roughgarden, J. 1995 Experimental removal of insectivores from rain forest canopy: direct and indirect 2776 S. D. Finkbeiner et al. Butterfly communal roosting on January 29, 2015http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from ente-Bosquets, J. & Chiao, C.-C. 2010 Positive selection of a duplicated UV-sensitive visual pigment coincides with wing pigment evolution in Heliconius butterflies. Proc. Natl Acad Sci. USA 107, 3628–3633. (doi:10. 1073/pnas.0910085107) 47 Chai, P. 1986 Field observations and feeding experiments on the responses of rufous-tailed jacamars (Galbula rufi- cauda) to free-flying butterflies in a tropical rainforest. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 29, 161–189. (doi:10.1111/j.1095- 8312.1986.tb01772.x) 48 Pinheiro, C. E. G. 2003 Does Mu¨llerian mimicry work in nature? Experiments with butterflies and birds (Tyranni- dae). Biotropica 35, 356–364. (doi:10.1111/j.1744-7429. 2003.tb00589.x) 49 Langham, G. M. 2004 Specialized avian predators repeatedly attack novel color morphs of Heliconius butter- flies. Evolution 58, 2783–2787. 50 Pinheiro, C. E. G. 2011 On the evolution of warning coloration, Batesian and Mu¨llerian mimicry in Neotro- pical butterflies: the role of jacamars (Galbulidae) and tyrant-flycatchers (Tyrannidae). J. Avian Biol. 42, 277–281. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-048X.2011.05435.x) 51 Kelber, A., Vorobyev, M. & Osorio, D. 2003 Animal colour vision: behavioural tests and physiological concepts. Biol. Rev. 78, 81–118. (doi:10.1017/S146479 3102005985) 52 Vorobyev, M. & Osorio, D. 1998 Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 351–358. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0302) 53 Bybee, S. M., Yuan, F., Ramstetter, M. D., Llorente- Bousquets, J., Reed, R. D., Osorio, D. & Briscoe, A. D. 2011 UV Photoreceptors and UV-yellow wing pigments in Heliconius butterflies allow a color signal to serve both mimicry and intraspecific communication. Am. Nat. 179, 38–51. (doi:10.1086/663192) 54 Ning, W., Zhang, Y. & Zheng, G. 2007 Home ranges and habitat vegetation characters in breeding season of Narcissus Flycatcher and Yellow-rumped Flycatcher.Proc. R. Soc. B (2012)59 Servedio, M. R. 2000 The effects of predator learning, forgetting, and recognition errors on the evolution of warning coloration. Evolution 54, 751–763. (doi:10. 1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00077.x) 60 Speed, M. P. 2000 Warning signals, receiver psychology and predator memory. Anim. Behav. 60, 269–278. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1430) 61 Ruxton, G. D., Sherratt, T. N. & Speed, M. P. 2004 Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 62 Brodie, E. D. 1993 Differential avoidance of coral snake banded patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica. Evolution 47, 227–235. (doi:10.2307/2410131) 63 Stobbe, N. & Schaefer, H. M. 2008 Enhancement of chromatic contrast increases predation risk for striped butterflies. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 1535–1541. (doi:10. 1098/rspb.2008.0209) 64 DesLauriers, J. V. & Francis, C. M. 1991 The effect of time of day on mist-net captures of passerines on spring migration. J. Field Ornithol. 62, 107–116. 65 Lindstedt, C., Huttunen, H., Kakko, M. & Mappes, J. 2011 Disentangling the evolution of weak warning sig- nals: high detection risk and low production costs of chemical defences in gregarious pine sawfly larvae. Evol. Ecol. 25, 1029–1046. (doi:10.1007/s10682-010- 9456-4) 66 Salcedo, C. 2006 Spatial dynamics of night roosting in Heliconius erato petiverana (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). MSc thesis, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. 67 Poulton, E. B. 1931 The gregarious sleeping habits of Heliconius charitonius L. Proc. R. Entomol. Soc. Lond. 6, 4–10. 68 Tuskes, P. M. & Brower, L. P. 1978 Overwintering ecol- ogy of the monarch butterfly, Danaus plexippus L., in California. Ecol. Entomol. 3, 141–153. (doi:10.1111/j. 1365-2311.1978.tb00912.x)Sison-Mangus, M. P., Reed, R. D., Warren, A. D., Llor- effects. Ecology 76, 1821–1834. (doi:10.2307/1940714)