October 2000] 1051Short Communications 1997. The relation among gonadal steroids, im- munocompetence, body mass, and behavior in young Black-headed Gulls (Larus ridibundus). American Naturalist 150:201?219. SAINO, N., A. P. M?LLER, AND A. M. BOLZERN. 1995. Testosterone effects on the immune system and parasite infestations in the Barn Swallow (Hi- rundo rustica): An experimental test of the im- munocompetence hypothesis. Behavioral Ecol- ogy 4:397?404. SAPOLSKY, R. M. 1992. Neuroendocrinology of the stress-response. Pages 287?324 in Behavioral en- docrinology (J. B. Becker, S. M. Breedlove, and D. Crews, Eds.). MIT Press, Cambridge, Massa- chusetts. SCHOECH, S. J., E. D. KETTERSON, AND V. NOLAN, JR. 1999. Exogenous testosterone and the adreno- cortical response in the Dark-eyed Junco, Junco hyemalis. Auk 116:64?72. SLATER, C. H., AND C. B. SCHRECK. 1993. Testosterone alters the immune response of chinook salmon Oncorhynchus tshawytscha. General and Compar- ative Endocrinology 89:291?298. ZUK, M., T. S. JOHNSEN, AND T. MACLARTY. 1995. En- docrine-immune interactions, ornaments and mate choice in Red Jungle Fowl. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B 260:205? 210. Received 6 August 1999, accepted 26 April 2000. Associate Editor: J. M. Wing?eld The Auk 117(4):1051?1056, 2000 Identi?cation of the Extinct Hawaiian Eagle (Haliaeetus) by mtDNA Sequence Analysis ROBERT C. FLEISCHER,1,2,5 STORRS L. OLSON,3 HELEN F. JAMES,3 AND ALAN C. COOPER2,4 1Department of Biological Sciences, University of Durham, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom; 2Molecular Genetics Laboratory, National Zoological Park, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.C. 20008, USA; 3Department of Vertebrate Zoology, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.C. 20560, USA; and 4Departments of Zoology and Biological Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX2 6QS, United Kingdom Among the many bones of extinct birds discovered in the ?rst extensive deposits studied in the Hawai- ian Islands were a few from Molokai and Oahu that clearly belonged to a large sea-eagle of the genus Haliaeetus, as shown by their large size and the fusion of the phalanges of the inner toe (Olson 1982, Olson and James 1982). Even with the discovery of a nearly complete skeleton of this eagle on Maui, its speci?c identi?cation could not be entirely resolved. Size and other characters eliminated most species of the ge- nus, but no qualitative osteological characters could be discerned between the Hawaiian fossils and avail- able skeletons of the Old World White-tailed Eagle (H. albicilla) and the New World Bald Eagle (H. leu- cocephalus; Olson and James 1991). Unable to make a morphological resolution, Olson and James (1991:62) were constrained to list this interesting specimen as ``Haliaeetus sp., aff. H. leucocephalus/H. albicilla.'' To identify the Hawaiian eagle more satisfactorily, we 5 Address correspondence to Molecular Genetics Laboratory, National Zoological Park, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.C. 20008, USA. E-mail: r?eischer@nzp.si.edu turned to ``ancient'' DNA methods to obtain mito- chondrial DNA (mtDNA) sequences from its bones. The bones used in ancient DNA analysis were ob- tained from a nearly complete skeleton found on 4 April 1988 in a good state of preservation in Puu Makua Cave, a lava tube at 1,463 m elevation on the south slope of Mt. Haleakala, Maui (Olson and James 1991). The eagle had died in a cave that served as a natural trap for ?ightless species such as moa-nalos, rails, and ibises. The only entrance to the cave is a skylight of approximately 5 m diameter above a ver- tical drop of approximately 22 m. The eagle may have ?own into the cave to scavenge ?ightless birds that had fallen in beforehand. The eagle presumably became trapped because the entrance was too small for such a large bird to be able to ?y back out. Previous analyses showed that the skeleton con- tained 4.1% nitrogen and amino acids in proportions only slightly different from those of modern cow bone (T. Stafford, Jr. pers. comm.). An AMS radio- carbon date on the skeleton gave an age of 3,300 6 60 years (two-sigma calibrated range 3,389 to 3,689 years; see James 1995). Because the nitrogen and ami- no acid content suggested that organic compounds 1052 [Auk, Vol. 117Short Communications were well preserved in the skeleton, and despite its antiquity, we chose this specimen for ancient DNA analysis. We isolated DNA from two bones from the skele- ton in two separate laboratories dedicated to ancient DNA analysis. DNA was ampli?ed from the Hawai- ian eagle bones for three different gene regions of mtDNA. We compared sequence from part of the cy- tochrome-b (cyt b) gene with cyt-b sequences ob- tained from GenBank for seven of the eight species of Haliaeetus known worldwide (Seibold and Helbig 1996). We also obtained sequences of the ATP sub- unit 8 gene (ATPase8) and part of the 12S ribosomal RNA gene (12S rRNA) and compared them with se- quences we obtained from individuals of H. leucoce- phalus, H. albicilla, and H. leucogaster. Methods.?Two bones were selected for analysis from the nearly complete skeleton found in Puu Makua Cave (USNM 431238): a thoracic rib fragment and a pedal phalanx. We obtained two tissue sam- ples of H. albicilla from the Swedish Museum of Nat- ural History (nos. 501 and 502) and feathers of H. al- bicilla and H. leucogaster (White-bellied Fish-Eagle) from captive birds in the United States. DNA sam- ples of H. leucocephalus were obtained from W. Piper. All pre-PCR protocols were conducted in isolated ancient DNA laboratories located in buildings dif- ferent from the ones containing the primary DNA laboratories. DNA isolations and PCRs for ATPase8 and 12S rRNA segments were conducted at the Na- tional Zoological Park (NZP) by A. C. Cooper, and DNA isolations and PCRs for cyt b and 12S rRNA were conducted at the University of Durham by R. Fleischer. Methods to reduce the probability of con- tamination followed Handt et al. (1994) and included the use of barrier pipet tips; sterile solutions; DNA destruction with UV illumination; liberal washing of equipment and bench surfaces with bleach solutions; and the use of extract, PCR, and ``large DNA'' con- trols. In addition, equipment and supplies were ded- icated to the ancient DNA laboratories, and work was not conducted in an ancient DNA laboratory after working in the main laboratory on the same day. Isolation protocols in the two laboratories were very similar. A small piece of each Haliaeetus bone was cut away and cleaned in sterile water. This was followed by a wash with rotation at room tempera- ture in sterile 0.5 M EDTA, after which the EDTA was discarded. At Durham, the bone was wrapped in heavy aluminum foil (which had been decontami- nated of DNA by UV illumination for 20 min) and crushed in a mortar and pestle (decontaminated by soaking in 50% bleach solution). At the NZP, a bleach-sterilized coffee grinder was used to pulver- ize the bone sample. The crushed bone was placed in 5 mL of sterile UV-treated lysis buffer (0.01 M Tris, ph 8.0; 0.002 M EDTA; 0.01 M NaCl; 10 mg/mL DTT; 1% SDS; and 1 mg/mL Proteinase K) and rotated overnight at 608C. The sample was then extracted twice with 5 mL of phenol and once with 5 mL of chloroform. The supernatant was placed in 1 mL of sterile water in a Centricon 30 microconcentrator and centrifuged until about 50 to 100 mL covered the membrane. A second 1 mL of sterile water was added and the centrifugation repeated. The remaining sam- ple was collected from the membrane by a short cen- trifugation and diluted to 200 mL total volume. From 2 to 4 mL of this were used in subsequent PCR ex- periments. We attempted PCR ampli?cations with primers that amplify different-sized segments of the three mtDNA genes: 12S rRNA, ATPase8, and cyt b. We de- signed a set of primers (AncEag. 2: forward, 59-AG- GAATCTGCCTACTAACACA-39, 39 is base 15,026 of Gallus gallus sequence; Desjardins and Morais 1990; reverse, 59-TTCGACATGTATGGGCTACG-39, 39 is base 15,090) from Haliaeetus eagle cyt-b sequence (Seibold and Helbig 1996) such that they ?anked a small (63 base pairs [bp]) region that contained four transitional substitutions between H. albicilla and H. leucocephalus and also differed by at least this much from the other ?ve Haliaeetus species. The AncEag. 2 product size was 104 bp. Primers cyt-b 2-rc (59-TGAGGACAAATATCC TTCTGAGG-39, 15,320 of Gallus, reverse comple- ment of cyt b 2; Kocher et al. 1989) and cyt-b ack (59- CCTCCTCAGGCTCATTCTAC-39, 15,376 of Gallus) ampli?ed a 98-bp segment of cyt b disjunct from AncEag. 2. The 12S rRNA segments were ampli?ed and sequenced using primers 12SA, 12SH, 12SJ, and 12SB2 (Cooper 1994), producing 392 bp of sequence. The ATPase8 segment included 35 bp of tRNA-lysine and was ampli?ed and sequenced with ``Birds-R-Us'' (59-TGGTCGAAGAAGCTTAGGTTCA-39, 9,241 of Gallus) and t-lys (59-CACCAGCACTAGCCTTTT AAG-39, 9,051 of Gallus). Final PCR components in a 50-mL reaction volume were 1X taq buffer (Perkin Elmer), 1.25 units of Taq- gold polymerase, 0.2 mM each dNTP, 2.0 mM MgCl2, 3 mg/mL non-acetylated BSA, and 1 mM of each primer. PCR was conducted for 45 cycles beginning with a ``hot-start'' for 10 min at 948C and ending with a 6-min soak at 728C. Conditions per cycle were 928C denaturation for 40 s, 508C annealing for 1 min, and 728C extension for 1 min. Successful ampli?cation was determined by visu- alizing PCR products on a 2% agarose gel. Single- banded products were cleaned by centrifugation us- ing a Qiagen kit following the manufacturer's in- structions. About 100 ng of product were used for cy- cle sequencing of both strands using dye-labeled dideoxynucleotide terminators; resulting sequence reactions were run on an ABI 377 sequencer. We as- sessed chromatograms for accuracy and aligned se- quences using Sequencher 3.0. We compared sequences from the Hawaiian eagle bones directly with those of other Haliaeetus species. We also conducted a total-evidence phylogenetic October 2000] 1053Short Communications T A B L E 1. Si xt y- ei gh t va ri ab le nu cl eo ti d e si te s fr om 70 4 si te s of m tD N A se qu en ce ob ta in ed fr om tw o bo n es of an ea g le sk el et on fo u n d in P u u M ak u a C av e, M au i, H aw ai i (U SN M 43 12 38 ) al ig n ed ag ai n st se qu en ce s of th re e sp ec ie s of H al ia ee tu s. O u tg ro u p se qu en ce s fo r cy t b ar e fr om Se ib ol d an d H el bi g (1 99 6) .G en B an k ac ce ss io n nu m be rs ar e X 86 75 3 fo r H .a lb ic il la ,Z 73 46 7 fo r H .l eu co ce ph al us ,a n d Z 73 46 8 fo r H .l eu co ga st er ;o u tg ro u p se qu en ce s fo r th e ot h er tw o g en es ar e fr om th is st u d y. N u cl eo ti d e si te s 1 to 15 ar e fr om 11 8 bp of cy t- b se qu en ce ,s it es 16 to 41 ar e fr om 39 2 nu cl eo ti d e si te s of 12 S rR N A se qu en ce ,a n d si te s 42 to 68 ar e fr om 19 4 nu cl eo ti d e si te s of A T Pa se 8 se qu en ce .D ot s in d ic at e id en ti ty of th e ba se w it h th e H aw ai ia n ea g le se qu en ce . Sa m p le 1 2 3 4 5 6 12 34 56 78 90 12 34 56 78 90 12 34 56 78 90 12 34 56 78 90 12 34 56 78 90 12 34 56 78 90 12 34 56 78 H aw ai ia n bo n e H .a lb ic il la H .l eu co ce ph al us H .l eu co ga st er T C C G T T T T T T C A A C A C A C A C T T C A A T T C T C G G C T C T G A G A A A G T C C A A C T A T T T A C A A A C T T A A A C G C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G . A . C . . A C . C . C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . G . . G G . A . . G . . . T . . C . . . . . . . . . . . . A . C T T A C C C C C C T C G T G T G T C A C C T G G C C T C T A A T C T C A . A G . . A G T T . G A C T C C . G T G G C T C C C G G T C G analysis of the sequences of the Hawaiian bone and composite sequences from three outgroup Haliaeetus taxa. Replicate sequences for H. leucocephalus and H. albicilla for ATPase8, and for H. leucocephalus for 12srRNA, were identical; thus, we used only one se- quence from these to represent each taxon in the analysis. The cyt-b sequences of H. albicilla, H. leu- cocephalus, and H. leucogaster were from a different study and therefore arise from different individuals than the sequences obtained for these same species in this study. Nonetheless, we combined the gene re- gions into the full 704 bp of sequence (shown abridged in Table 1 as variable sites only) and con- ducted an exhaustive search, with no character weighting, in PAUP* (Swofford 1999). We also ran a bootstrap (1,000 replications) using a branch-and- bound search algorithm to determine the relative support for different clades. In addition, we con- strained the Hawaiian bone to be a sister taxon to H. albicilla in one tree and H. leucocephalus in another and then used a Kishino-Hasegawa test (Kishino and Hasegawa 1989) to determine whether the alterna- tive trees differed signi?cantly in length. Results.?We obtained a total of 704 nucleotide sites of sequence from the Hawaiian eagle bones: 392 nucleotide sites of 12S rRNA, 118 bp of cyt b, and 194 bp of ATPase8 (Table 1). We compared the cyt-b se- quences with those obtained by Seibold and Helbig (1996) for seven species of Haliaeetus, and the 12S and ATPase8 sequences with ones we obtained from H. albicilla, H. leucocephalus, and H. leucogaster (Table 1). The cyt-b sequence of the Hawaiian Haliaeetus was identical to that of H. albicilla and differed by 5 to 17 bp from all other Haliaeetus taxa, including H. leu- cocephalus. The more-conserved 12S sequence was identical to that of H. albicilla but differed by two transitions from the sequence for H. leucocephalus and by 24 substitutions from H. leucogaster. The Hawaiian eagle bone ATPase8 sequence differed at only 3 po- sitions from H. albicilla, but differed at 6 positions from H. leucocephalus and at 24 positions from H. leu- cogaster. In summary, the Hawaiian Haliaeetus bone se- quences differed at only 3 of 704 bp (0.4 %) from the White-tailed Eagle, but by 13 of 704 (1.8 %) from the Bald Eagle. Divergence of the Hawaiian eagle bone sequence from the Bald Eagle sequence was signi?- cantly greater than its divergence from the White- tailed Eagle sequence (Fisher exact test, P 5 0.021). Divergence of the Hawaiian bone sequence from the White-tailed Eagle sequence was not signi?cantly different from zero (Fisher exact test, P 5 0.250). The three ATPase8 sequence differences between the Hawaiian eagle skeleton and White-tailed Eagle sequences included two A-G transitions and one A- T transversion (Table 1). Because of the rarity of A-T transversions in mtDNA at this level of divergence, and the fact that the T site of the latter was unique to the Hawaiian eagle bone, it is possible that it repre- 1054 [Auk, Vol. 117Short Communications FIG. 1. Total-evidence parsimony phylogram showing relationships among mtDNA sequences from Ha- waiian bone and from three species of Haliaeetus (see Table 1). Note that the outgroup sequences are chimeric for individuals: ATPase8 and 12S rRNA were obtained from individuals sequenced as part of this study, and cyt-b sequences are from different individuals described in Seibold and Helbig (1996). The 98% bootstrap value for the Hawaiian bone and H. albicilla clade is on the clade's node. sents a postmortem artifact rather than a real sub- stitution. Other than this single change, which does not modify our conclusions, no other differences are suggestive of artifacts owing to environmental mod- i?cation. The reconstructed phylogeny (Fig. 1) shows the close relationship of the Hawaiian bone sequences to those of H. albicilla and their more-distant relation- ship to H. leucocephalus and the outgroup taxon H. leucogaster. Bootstrap support for a Hawaiian bone/ H. albicilla clade was very high (98%). Constraining the Hawaiian bone sequences to be sister to H. leu- cocephalus increased the tree length by ?ve steps, but the Kishino-Hasegawa test was not quite signi?cant in supporting the Hawaiian bone/H. albicilla clade over the Hawaiian bone/H. leucocephalus clade (t 5 1.9, P 5 0.058). Discussion.?We obtained mtDNA sequences from two subfossil Hawaiian eagle bones that are nearly identical to ones from Haliaeetus albicilla. Our anal- yses were conducted in two separate laboratories us- ing independently purchased supplies, solutions, and synthesized primers. No outgroup samples of Haliaeetus were present in the laboratories used at Durham University (where the cyt-b and some 12SrRNA sequences were obtained). In the NZP lab- oratory (where the ATPase8 and 12SrRNA sequences were obtained), all outgroups were analyzed after DNA isolation from the subfossil. Thus, it is extreme- ly unlikely that these sequences are the result of con- tamination by external sources of DNA. It is remarkable that DNA and amino acids are well preserved in this skeleton that has lain in a cave in a subtropical climate for perhaps 3,500 years, espe- cially when considering that the eagle skeleton was exposed to dripping water in the cave and was moist when collected. Factors that may have favored pres- ervation of organic compounds include the relatively constant, cool temperatures of this high-elevation cave, and the fact that the eagle corpse was always protected from UV radiation, being preserved in darkness. Our results show that DNA sequences of the Ha- waiian Haliaeetus are more similar to sequences of the White-tailed Eagle than they are to those of the Bald Eagle. They do not support a hypothesis that the Hawaiian eagle skeleton is conspeci?c with the Bald Eagle. The protein-coding sequence divergence (i.e. cyt b and ATPase8, combined) between the two (3.5%) is higher than might be expected for a within- species level divergence in birds and more typical of divergence between congeneric species (Klicka and Zink 1997, Avise and Walker 1998). On the other hand, the divergence between the White-tailed Eagle and Hawaiian bone protein-coding sequences (1.0%) is much lower and is within the typical range of within-species variation. Our results do not allow us to reject the hypothesis that the Hawaiian skeleton is that of a White-tailed Eagle, or its very close relative. Therefore, we suggest that the Hawaiian eagle is con- speci?c with and represents a disjunct Hawaiian population of the White-tailed Eagle. The similarity of the sampled sequence from the Hawaiian eagle to that of the White-tailed Eagle also suggests that this species colonized the archipelago at a relatively late date, although one that apparently October 2000] 1055Short Communications preceded the arrival of humans (James 1995). The relative recency of colonization is also supported by the absence of eagle fossils in the sediments of a Pleistocene lake at Ulupau Head, Oahu (James 1987) and the lack of skeletal differences between the Ha- waiian eagle and H. albicilla (Olson and James 1991). The Hawaiian eagle and the Black-crowned Night- Heron (Nycticorax nycticorax) are the only lineages of Hawaiian birds that successfully colonized the ar- chipelago without the aid of humans, but that have not differentiated at least to the level of subspecies. This, and other evidence (e.g. Olson and James 1991, Fleischer and McIntosh 2000) suggest that coloniza- tion of the Hawaiian Islands is an ongoing process and that ``equilibrium'' diversities predicted by the- ories of island biogeography have not yet been reached. Although the eagle appears to have been the only indigenous predator capable of depredating the large ?ightless waterfowl of the islands (see Olson and James 1991), genetic and other data indicate that these waterfowl existed in the islands well before the arrival of H. albicilla (Paxinos 1998, Sorenson et al. 1999). Our study demonstrates the utility of analyzing fossil or ancient DNA for identifying species when other approaches will not work. The identi?cation of the Hawaiian eagle adds another Palearctic element to the sources of colonization of the Hawaiian avi- fauna. Most avian lineages in Hawaii appear to have colonized from the New World, particularly North America, and very few from the Palearctic (Mayr 1943, Fleischer and McIntosh 2000). Other lineages of Palearctic origin include the Laysan Rail (Porzana pal- meri; Olson 1973a,b; Fleischer and McIntosh 2000) and possibly the Acrocephalus warblers and the dre- panidines (Groth 1994, Fleischer et al. 1998). Histor- ical records of eagles in the Hawaiian chain include a Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos) resident on Kauai for 17 years (Berger 1981, Pyle 1997) that may have originated in either North America or Asia, and the Asian Steller's Sea-Eagle (H. pelagicus) that was re- corded from the Northwestern Hawaiian Islands of Kure and Midway (Pyle 1997). Haliaeetus albicilla is a latitudinal migrant in Eurasia, especially in the east- ern part of its range (del Hoyo et al. 1994), and some- times is a vagrant and thus could be expected to have reached the islands. Acknowledgments.?We thank R. Michael Severns and Pauline Fiene-Severns for guiding us to Puu Makua Cave and helping to collect the eagle skele- ton. C. Pickett (National Zoological Park), P. Erickson (Swedish Museum of Natural History), and W. Piper generously contributed samples of outgroup Haliaee- tus, and R. Hoelzel and C. Shaw provided lab facili- ties in Durham. We also thank Thomas Stafford, Jr., for helping to collect the skeleton and for providing results of his CHN, amino acid, and radiocarbon analyses. We thank R. Chandler, R. Prum, and an anonymous reviewer for comments on the manu- script, including the request for phylogenetic anal- ysis. The research was supported by the National Geographic Society; Friends of the National Zoo; and the Wetmore, Walcott, and Scholarly Studies Funds of the Smithsonian Institution. LITERATURE CITED AVISE, J. C., AND D. WALKER. 1998. Pleistocene phy- logeographic effects on avian populations and the speciation process. Proceedings of the Royal Society London Series B 265:457?463. BERGER, A. J. 1981. Hawaiian birdlife, 2nd ed. Uni- versity of Hawaii Press, Honolulu. COOPER, A. C. 1994. DNA from museum specimens. Pages 149?165 in Ancient DNA (B. Herrmann and S. Hummel, Eds.). Springer-Verlag, New York. DEL HOYO, J., A. ELLIOTT, AND J. SARGATAL (Eds.). 1994. Handbook of the birds of the world, vol. 2. New World vultures to guineafowl. Lynx Edi- cions, Barcelona. DESJARDINS, P., AND R. MORAIS. 1990. Sequence and gene organization of the chicken mitochondrial genome: A novel gene order in higher verte- brates. Journal of Molecular Biology 212:599? 634. FLEISCHER, R. C., AND C. E. MCINTOSH. 2000. Molec- ular systematics and biogeography of the Ha- waiian avifauna. In press in Status, ecology, and evolution of the Hawaiian avifauna. Studies in Avian Biology. FLEISCHER, R. C., C. E. MCINTOSH, AND C. L. TARR. 1998. Evolution on a volcanic conveyor belt: Us- ing phylogeographic reconstructions and K-Ar based ages of the Hawaiian Islands to estimate molecular evolutionary rates. Molecular Ecology 7:533?545. GROTH, J. G. 1994. A mitochondrial cytochrome b phylogeny of cardueline ?nches. Journal fu? r Or- nithology 135(Sonderheft):31. HANDT, O., M. HO? SS, M. KRINGS, AND S. PA? A? BO. 1994. Ancient DNA: Methodological challenges. Ex- perientia 50:524?529. JAMES, H. F. 1987. A late Pleistocene avifauna from the island of Oahu, Hawaiian Islands. Docu- ments des Laboratoires de Geologie de la Faculte des Sciences de Lyon 99:221?230. JAMES, H. F. 1995. Prehistoric extinctions and ecolog- ical changes on oceanic islands. Ecological Stud- ies 115:88?102. KISHINO, H., AND M. HASEGAWA. 1989. Evaluation of the maximum likelihood estimate of evolution- ary tree topologies from DNA sequence data, and the branching order in the Hominoidea. Journal of Molecular Evolution 29:170?179. KLICKA, J., AND R. M. ZINK. 1997. The importance of 1056 [Auk, Vol. 117Short Communications recent ice ages in speciation: A failed paradigm. Science 277:1666?1669. KOCHER, T. D., W. K. THOMAS, A. MEYER, S. V. ED- WARDS, S. PAABO, F. X. VILLABLANCA, AND A. C. WILSON. 1989. Dynamics of mitochondrial DNA evolution in mammals: Ampli?cation and se- quencing with conserved primers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 86: 6196?6200. MAYR, E. 1943. The zoogeographic position of the Hawaiian Islands. Condor 45:45?48. OLSON, S. L. 1973a. Evolution of the rails of the South Atlantic islands (Aves: Rallidae). Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology 152:1?53. OLSON, S. L. 1973b. A classi?cation of the Rallidae. Wilson Bulletin 85:381?446. OLSON, S. L. 1982. The distribution of fused phalan- ges of the inner toe in the Accipitridae. Bulletin of the British Ornithologists' Club 102:8?12. OLSON, S. L., AND H. F. JAMES. 1982. Prodromus of the fossil avifauna of the Hawaiian Islands. Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology No. 365. OLSON, S. L., AND H. F. JAMES. 1991. Descriptions of 32 new species of birds from the Hawaiian Is- lands: Part I, Non-passeriformes. Ornithological Monographs No. 45. PAXINOS, E. E. 1998. Prehistoric anseriform diversity in the Hawaiian Islands: A molecular perspec- tive from the analysis of subfossil DNA. Ph.D. dissertation, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island. PYLE, R. L. 1997. Checklist of the birds of Hawaii. 1997. Elepaio 57:129?138. SEIBOLD, I., AND A. J. HELBIG. 1996. Phylogenetic re- lationships of the sea eagles (genus Haliaeetus): Reconstructions based on morphology, allo- zymes and mitochondrial DNA sequence. Jour- nal of Zoological Systematics and Evolution Re- search 34:103?112. SORENSON, M. D., A. C. COOPER, T. A. QUINN, H. F. JAMES, S. OLSON, AND R. C. FLEISCHER. 1999. Evolutionary relationships of the extinct moa nalos of Hawaii based on ancient DNA. Proceed- ings of the Royal Society of London Series B 266: 2187?2194. SWOFFORD, D. L. 1999. PAUP*. Phylogenetic analysis using parsimony (*and other methods), version 4.0d65. Sinauer and Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts. Received 27 May 1999, accepted 27 April 2000. Associate Editor: R. O. Prum The Auk 117(4):1056?1060, 2000 Are Nest Predation and Brood Parasitism Correlated in Yellow Warblers? A Test of the Cowbird Predation Hypothesis CELIA M. MCLAREN AND SPENCER G. SEALY1 Department of Zoology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater) are gen- eralist brood parasites that reduce the productivity of many of their passerine hosts through egg removal and competition between cowbird and host nestlings (Lowther 1993, Payne 1998, Lorenzana and Sealy 1999). Recently, it has been suggested that cowbirds exert a previously unappreciated in?uence on the de- mography of their hosts through predation (Arcese et al. 1992, 1996; Smith and Arcese 1994; Arcese and Smith 1999). The cowbird predation hypothesis (Ar- cese et al. 1992) was suggested to explain the fre- quently observed link between nest predation and interspeci?c brood parasitism in many passerines (e.g. Wolf 1987, Payne and Payne 1998). Speci?cally, the hypothesized link is a direct result of cowbird 1 Address correspondence to this author. E-mail: sgsealy@cc.umanitoba.ca predation of host nests that are discovered too late in the host's nesting cycle for successful parasitism. This creates future opportunities for parasitism by causing hosts to renest (Arcese et al. 1996). Alter- natively, a link between predation and parasitism may result if cowbird parasitism facilitates preda- tion, or if both factors are correlated with a third var- iable such as nest vulnerability (Arcese and Smith 1999). Determining the independent effects of brood parasitism and nest predation on host populations is dif?cult because of this potential functional depen- dence (Pease and Grzybowski 1995, Arcese and Smith 1999). In a resident population of Song Sparrows (Melos- piza melodia) on Mandarte Island, British Columbia, the frequency of nest failure was positively related to the frequency of cowbird parasitism (Arcese et al. 1996). In 5 of 19 years when cowbirds were rare, ab- sent, or removed on the island, nest failure was lower October 2000] 1057Short Communications than when one or more cowbirds were present on a consistent basis. To explain these trends, Arcese et al. (1992) proposed the cowbird predation hypothesis, which they subsequently tested directly (Arcese et al. 1996). A critical prediction of the hypothesis was supported when Arcese et al. (1996) found that par- asitized nests failed less often than unparasitized nests when female cowbirds maintained exclusive territories, because cowbirds should regularly dep- redate unparasitized nests but not nests they have al- ready parasitized. To assess whether results from this island popu- lation were unique, J. N. M. Smith and M. J. Taitt (un- publ. data) and Rogers et al. (1997) analyzed com- parable data from Song Sparrows on Westham Is- land, near mainland British Columbia. In both stud- ies, lower cowbird numbers were associated with higher host nesting success, as predicted. However, Rogers et al. (1997) found no difference in the fre- quency of nest failure in parasitized and unparasit- ized nests, contrary to the key prediction of the hy- pothesis. Published analyses of potential correlates of cowbird predation behavior are limited to these populations of Song Sparrows. Arcese et al. (1996) cited several anecdotal accounts of cowbirds de- stroying passerine nests and also called for addition- al independent tests of the cowbird predation hy- pothesis (see also Arcese and Smith 1999). We compared the frequency of failure of parasit- ized versus unparasitized nests in a population of Yellow Warblers (Dendroica petechia) in the Delta Marsh region of southern Manitoba, using data col- lected from 1974 to 1976. The cowbird predation hy- pothesis predicts that unparasitized nests should fail more often than parasitized nests when cowbird egg-laying ranges do not overlap. In contrast, para- sitized nests may fail more often than unparasitized nests if cowbird egg-laying ranges overlap, or if cow- birds facilitate rather than cause predation. We also examined the evidence in support of the prediction that unparasitized hosts whose nests are depredated should have a high probability of their replacement nests being parasitized. We further tested the general hypothesis that nest predation is linked to brood par- asitism, either directly or through facilitation, by ex- amining the relationship between frequency of nest predation and parasitism in seven Yellow Warbler populations across North America (cf. Arcese and Smith 1999). Yellow Warblers are a common host of the cowbird at Delta Marsh (19.1% of 2,163 nests parasitized; Neudorf and Sealy 1994) and elsewhere. They typi- cally accept cowbird eggs laid after the midpoint of the laying stage but respond to eggs laid earlier ei- ther by burial of the cowbird egg (and any host eggs) under a new nest, or by desertion followed by re- nesting at a different site (Clark and Robertson 1981, Sealy 1995). Methods.?During 1974 to 1976, Yellow Warblers at Delta Marsh suffered high cowbird parasitism and high predation. Further details of the study site, spe- cies, and methods used to ?nd and follow nests are given in Goossen (1978) and Sealy (1995). We ex- amined data for 331 Yellow Warbler nesting at- tempts. Parasitism frequency (proportion of all nests that received one or more cowbird eggs) was 26%. We calculated the frequency of failure of parasitized and unparasitized nests in the subset of nests sur- viving at least to the second day of incubation to eliminate the bias of unequal exposure to parasitism. Nest-failure frequency is de?ned as the proportion of all nests that ?edged no young. A parasitized nest in which the cowbird egg (and Yellow Warbler eggs, if present) was buried and a new clutch was laid was considered a single parasitized nesting attempt. Where possible, we identi?ed cause of nest failure for each unsuccessful nesting attempt. For example, weather or predation could be implicated con?dently in many cases. Only nests that clearly failed as a re- sult of loss of most or all of the eggs or nestlings were considered to have been depredated. Nesting stage at which failure occurred was as- signed on the basis of laying date of the ?rst egg for nests found during building or laying or by back- dating from the ?rst day of incubation (i.e. the day after the last warbler egg is laid) or from the date of hatching for nests found later. For nests that were partially depredated over several days, the stage at the ?rst day of egg or nestling loss was considered the stage of predation. Common nest predators at Delta Marsh include red squirrels (Tamiasciurus hud- sonicus), Common Grackles (Quiscalus quiscala), Yel- low-headed Blackbirds (Xanthocephalus xanthocephal- us), Red-winged Blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus), Baltimore Orioles (Icterus galbula), and Gray Cat- birds (Dumetella carolinensis; see Sealy 1994). We used corrected x2 tests to compare the frequen- cy of failure of parasitized and unparasitized nests, combining data for all years. We also performed an additional test con?ned to nests for which failure could be con?dently attributed to predation. We ex- amined the relationship between parasitism and pre- dation in seven populations of Yellow Warblers from Ontario (S. Yezerinac unpubl. data), Michigan (Batts 1958, DellaSala 1985), Colorado (C. Ortega and J. Or- tega unpubl. data, Howe and Knopf 2000), Montana (J. Tewksbury unpubl. data) and Manitoba (this study). We used Spearman rank correlation, weight- ed by sample size, to determine whether the per- centage of nests depredated was related to the per- centage of nests parasitized. Results and Discussion.?The frequencies of failure of parasitized and unparasitized nests were opposite to predictions of the cowbird predation hypothesis: parasitized nests failed more often (52% of 64 nests) than unparasitized nests (37% of 214 nests; x2 5 3.81, P 5 0.05). To assess whether predation and parasit- ism are linked, we compared the frequency of failure 1058 [Auk, Vol. 117Short Communications of parasitized and unparasitized nests for failures that we could con?dently attribute to predation. Again, the trend was opposite to predictions, al- though the difference was not statistically signi?cant (40% of 52 parasitized nests vs. 30% of 192 unpara- sitized nests depredated; x2 5 1.69, P 5 0.19). These results did not support the cowbird predation hy- pothesis. In contrast to Arcese et al.'s (1996) results, unpar- asitized nests were less likely to fail than were par- asitized nests. This result was not unexpected be- cause Yellow Warblers frequently respond to para- sitism by nest desertion (Clark and Robertson 1981, Burgham and Picman 1989, Sealy 1995). However, because we considered only nests that failed after the window for parasitism (after the second day of in- cubation), any nests deserted in response to parasit- ism within this window were not included. Similarly, Payne and Payne (1998) found that parasitized In- digo Bunting (Passerina cyanea) nests failed more of- ten than unparasitized nests. This result may occur if cowbirds facilitate predation by their activities near host nests (Arcese and Smith 1999). However, because the frequency of failure was not signi?cantly higher in parasitized nests, we have no evidence to support the cowbird facilitation hypothesis. Identi?cation of nest predators is crucial to under- standing the association between nest failure and brood parasitism. Field signs after nest predation only occasionally are useful in identifying nest pred- ators (Arcese and Smith 1999, Thompson et al. 1999); hence, we have no clear record of whether cowbirds or some other predator were responsible for nesting failures. However, many nest predators exist in the study area, and of several observed predation events at Yellow Warbler nests, none involved cowbirds (Sealy 1994). In a similar habitat, Thompson et al. (1999) documented only one case out of 25 predation events recorded on video of nest predation by a cow- bird. We conclude that cowbirds probably were not responsible for many of the predation events at the Yellow Warbler nests in our sample. The critical prediction of the cowbird predation hypothesis is that parasitized nests should fail less often than unparasitized nests, but this prediction would hold only when individual female cowbirds have distinct egg-laying ranges, because a female should destroy unparasitized nests but not nests she has parasitized (Arcese et al. 1996). When female cowbirds defend exclusive intraspeci?c nesting ter- ritories, any parasitized nest a female encounters is likely to contain her own egg. On the other hand, if cowbird egg-laying ranges overlap, different females would be likely to discover the same nests if some nests are especially conspicuous to cowbirds in gen- eral, and subsequent females may depredate a par- asitized nest. Therefore, parasitized nests should fail more often than unparasitized nests (Arcese et al. 1996). One could argue that the trend we documented re- sulted from overlap of egg-laying ranges among fe- male cowbirds. However, recent DNA work has shown that egg-laying ranges of female cowbirds do not overlap at Delta Marsh (Alderson et al. 1999), and we have no reason to believe that this was not the case 25 years ago. Indeed, egg-laying ranges like- ly overlapped less often then, because Yellow War- bler nesting density was about twice what it is at pre- sent, but cowbird densities were similar (S. Sealy un- publ. data). If host nests are abundant, female cow- birds should not need to search wide areas for host nests. The low frequency of multiple parasitism in the Yellow Warbler data set (8 of 85 parasitized nests received more than one cowbird egg) also supports the claim that egg-laying ranges overlapped little. Yellow Warblers frequently respond to parasitism by deserting (burying) the cowbird egg. In the pre- sent sample, burial occurred at 39% of parasitized nests. A nest at which burial has occurred and a new clutch is laid may suffer increased exposure to pre- dation compared with a nest that proceeds directly to incubation. This difference would bias our results against the prediction of higher frequency of failure of unparasitized nests. When we excluded nests at which burial had occurred, our results were un- changed. In addition, the frequency of nesting failure did not differ between parasitized nests in which cowbird eggs were buried versus accepted (x2 5 3.02, P 5 0.08), and the trend was opposite to expectation because ``burial'' nests failed less often (36% of 25 nests) than ``acceptance'' nests (62% of 39 nests). Predation of a parasitized nest in which the cow- bird egg is buried is predicted from the cowbird pre- dation hypothesis only if cowbird egg-laying ranges overlap. Otherwise, a female probably remembers the nests she has parasitized (Sherry et al. 1993), and predation of these nests would be a form of the ``Ma- ?a'' behavior described in parasitic cuckoos (Zahavi 1979, Soler et al. 1995). If Ma?a-like behavior occurs, failures due to predation of parasitized Yellow War- bler nests should occur more often when egg rejec- tion (i.e. burial) has taken place than when it has not. This was not the case in our study. Another prediction of the cowbird predation hy- pothesis is that unparasitized hosts whose nests are depredated should have a high probability of their replacement nest being parasitized. We could not test this prediction because the identities of Yellow Warblers that renested were not known. However, in ?ve cases where a ?rst nest was depredated (two during laying, three during incubation), the same nests were reused for the new nesting attempts, pre- sumably by the same adults. None of these replace- ment clutches was parasitized. We present two explanations for our failure to sup- port predictions of the cowbird predation hypothe- sis. First, host density is high at Delta Marsh, and op- portunities for parasitism should not be limiting. In October 2000] 1059Short Communications FIG. 1. Percent of nests depredated versus per- cent parasitized in seven populations of Yellow War- blers, weighted by the number of nests studied. Size of open circles indicates sample sizes for each pop- ulation: Ontario (ON, n 5 74; S. Yezerinac unpubl. data); Manitoba (MB, n 5 331; this study); northern Colorado (nCO, n 5 73; Howe and Knopf 2000); southwestern Colorado (swCO, n 5 93; C. Ortega and J. Ortega unpubl. data); southern Michigan (sMI, n 5 20; Batts 1958); Montana (MO, n 5 358; J. Tewks- bury unpubl. data); southeastern Michigan (seMI, n 5 25; DellaSala 1985). contrast, this may not be the case on Mandarte Is- land, where few alternate host species occurred. However, Smith and Taitt (unpubl. data) document- ed further support for the cowbird predation hy- pothesis in Song Sparrows on Westham Island, Brit- ish Columbia, where alternate hosts were present. Despite this range of opportunity, alternate hosts are rarely used on Westham Island (J. N. M. Smith pers. comm.). Second, the timing of breeding of alternate host species is less synchronous at Delta Marsh than on Mandarte Island, and the presence of hosts late in the breeding season means that opportunities for parasitism are greater there. On Mandarte Island, cowbirds sometimes continue to lay until the end of the Song Sparrow laying season (Smith and Arcese 1994), but after this date, they are rarely seen on the island. At Delta Marsh, however, several other fre- quently parasitized hosts continue to breed later in the season, including Song Sparrows that lay second clutches. Both of the explanations noted above pre- suppose that nest predation confers a cost for cow- birds. If a cowbird incurs no cost by depredating a host nest, nests discovered too late for parasitism should always be destroyed regardless of the num- ber of opportunities for parasitism. Surprisingly, we found that across seven popula- tions of Yellow Warblers, predation and parastism were signi?cantly negatively correlated (rs 5 20.85, P 5 0.008; Fig. 1), in contrast to what has typically been documented (e.g. Johnson and Temple 1990, Donovan et al 1997, Arcese and Smith 1999). We sug- gest that the frequently observed link between pre- dation and parasitism cannot be explained by the cowbird predation hypothesis or the cowbird facili- tation hypothesis, but instead represents a coinci- dental preference by nest predators and brood par- asites for particular habitat features (Donovan et al. 1997, Tewksbury et al 1998). The only way to sepa- rate the independent in?uence of cowbirds as pred- ators is to compare predation frequencies in popu- lations where cowbirds are naturally or experimen- tally variable in abundance (Arcese and Smith 1999). In Song Sparrows, this test suggests that changes in the frequency of parasitism are directly linked to changes in the frequency of nest predation (Arcese and Smith 1999). Further experiments on other lo- cally abundant hosts, such as Yellow Warblers, are needed. Acknowledgments.?We thank J. P. Goossen for col- lecting the original data and the staff of the Univer- sity of Manitoba Field Station at Delta Marsh for lo- gistical support. The comments of P. Arcese, A. L. A. Middleton, S. I. Rothstein, J. N. M. Smith, J. J. Tewks- bury, and an anonymous reviewer greatly improved the manuscript. We also thank C. P. Ortega, J. N. M. Smith, J. J. Tewksbury, and S. Yezerinac for kindly supplying unpublished data. Funds were provided by a research grant to SGS from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. LITERATURE CITED ALDERSON, G. W., H. L. GIBBS, AND S. G. SEALY. 1999. Determining the reproductive behaviour of in- dividual Brown-headed Cowbirds using micro- satellite DNA markers. Animal Behaviour 58: 895?905. ARCESE, P., AND J. N. M. SMITH. 1999. Impacts of nest predation and brood parasitism on the produc- tivity of North American passerines. Pages 2953?2966 in Proceedings of the 22nd Interna- tional Ornithological Congress (N. J. Adams and R. H. Slotow, Eds.). Durban, 1998. BirdLife South Africa, Johannesburg. ARCESE, P., J. N. M. SMITH, AND M. I. HATCH. 1996. Nest predation by cowbirds and its consequenc- es for passerine demography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 93:4608? 4611. ARCESE, P., J. N. M. SMITH, W. M. HOCHACHKA, C. M. ROGERS, AND D. LUDWIG. 1992. Stability, regu- lation, and the determination of abundance in an insular Song Sparrow population. Ecology 73: 805?822. BATTS, H. L. 1958. The distribution and population of nesting birds on a farm in southern Michigan. Jack-Pine Warbler 36:131?149. BURGHAM, M. C. J., AND J. PICMAN. 1989. Effect of 1060 [Auk, Vol. 117Short Communications Brown-headed Cowbirds on the evolution of Yel- low Warbler anti-parasite strategies. Animal Be- haviour 38:298?308. CLARK, K. L., AND R. J. ROBERTSON. 1981. Cowbird parasitism and the evolution of anti-parasite strategies in the Yellow Warbler. Wilson Bulletin 93:249?258. DELLASALA, D. A. 1985. The Yellow Warbler in south- eastern Michigan: Factors affecting its produc- tivity. Jack-Pine Warbler 63:52?60. DONOVAN, T. M., P. W. JONES, E. M. ANNAND, AND F. R. THOMPSON III. 1997. Variation in local-scale edge effects: Mechanisms and landscape con- text. Ecology 78:2064?2075. GOOSSEN, J. P. 1978. Breeding biology and reproduc- tive success of the Yellow Warbler on the Delta Beach Ridge, Manitoba. M.S. thesis, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg. HOWE, W. H., AND F. L. KNOPF. 2000. The role of veg- etation in cowbird parasitism of Yellow War- blers. Pages 200?203 in Ecology and manage- ment of cowbirds and their hosts (J. N. M Smith, T. L. Cook, S. I. Rothstein, S. K. Robinson, and S. G. Sealy, Eds.). University of Texas Press, Austin. JOHNSON, R. G., AND S. A. TEMPLE. 1990. Nest pre- dation and brood parasitism of tallgrass prairie birds. Journal of Wildlife Management 54:106? 111. LORENZANA, J. C., AND S.G. SEALY. 1999. A meta- analysis of the impact of parasitism by the Brown-headed Cowbird on its hosts. Pages 241? 253 in Research and management of the Brown- headed Cowbird in western landscapes (M. L. Morrison, L. S. Hall, S. K. Robinson, S. I. Roth- stein, D. C. Hahn, and T. D. Rich, Eds.). Studies in Avian Biology No. 18. LOWTHER, P. E. 1993. Brown-headed Cowbird (Mol- othrus ater). In The birds of North America, no. 47. (A. Poole and F. Gill, Eds.). Academy of Nat- ural Sciences, Philadelphia, and American Or- nithologists' Union, Washington, D.C. NEUDORF, D. L., AND S. G. SEALY. 1994. Sunrise nest attentiveness in cowbird hosts. Condor 96:162? 169. PAYNE, R. B. 1998. Brood parasitism in birds: Strang- ers in the nest. BioScience 48:377?386. PAYNE, R. B., AND L. L. PAYNE. 1998. Brood parasit- ism by cowbirds: Risks and effects on reproduc- tive success and survival in Indigo Buntings. Be- havioral Ecology 9:64?73. PEASE, C. M., AND J. A. GRZYBOWSKI. 1995. Assessing the consequences of brood parasitism and nest predation on seasonal fecundity in passerine birds. Auk 112:343?363. ROGERS, C. M., M. J. TAITT, J. N. M. SMITH, AND G. JONGEJAN. 1997. Nest predation and cowbird parasitism create a demographic sink in wet- land-breeding Song Sparrows. Condor 99:622? 633. SEALY, S. G. 1994. Observed acts of egg destruction, egg removal, and predation on nests of passerine birds at Delta Marsh, Manitoba. Canadian Field- Naturalist 108:41?51. SEALY, S. G. 1995. Burial of cowbird eggs by parasit- ized Yellow Warblers: An empirical and experi- mental study. Animal Behaviour 49:877?889. SHERRY, D. F., M. R. L. FORBES, M. KHURGEL, AND G. O. IVY. 1993. Females have a larger hippocampus than males in the brood-parasitic Brown-headed Cowbird. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 90:7839?7843. SMITH, J. N. M., AND P. ARCESE. 1994. Brown-headed Cowbirds and an island population of Song Sparrows: A 16-year study. Condor 96:916?934 SOLER, M., J. J. SOLER, J. G. MARTINEZ, AND A. P. M?LLER. 1995. Magpie host manipulation by Great Spotted Cuckoos: Evidence for an avian Ma?a? Evolution 49:770?775. TEWKSBURY, J. J., S. J. HEJL, AND T. E. MARTIN. 1998. Breeding productivity does not decline with in- creasing fragmentation in a western landscape. Ecology 79:2890?2903. THOMPSON, F. R. III, W. DIJAK, AND D. E. BURHANS. 1999. Video identi?cation of predators at song- bird nests in old ?elds. Auk 116:259?264. WOLF, L. 1987. Host-parasite interactions of Brown- headed Cowbirds and Dark-eyed Juncos in Vir- ginia. Wilson Bulletin 99:338?350. ZAHAVI, A. 1979. Parasitism and nest predation in parasitic cuckoos. American Naturalist 113:157? 159. Received 27 September 1999, accepted 28 April 2000. Associate Editor: S. I. Rothstein