on March 22, 2017http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.orgResearch Cite this article: Ralls K, McInerney NR, Gagne RB, Ernest HB, Tinker MT, Fujii J, Maldonado J. 2017 Mitogenomes and relatedness do not predict frequency of tool-use by sea otters. Biol. Lett. 13: 20160880. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2016.0880Received: 7 November 2016 Accepted: 28 February 2017Subject Areas: behaviour, ecology, evolution Keywords: tool, sea otter, mitogenome, diet specializationAuthor for correspondence: Katherine Ralls e-mail: rallsk@thegrid.netElectronic supplementary material is available online at https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.fig- share.c.3711958.& 2017 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.Evolutionary biology Mitogenomes and relatedness do not predict frequency of tool-use by sea otters Katherine Ralls1, Nancy Rotzel McInerney1, Roderick B. Gagne2, Holly B. Ernest2, M. Tim Tinker3, Jessica Fujii4 and Jesus Maldonado1 1Center for Conservation Genomics, Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, Washington, DC 20008, USA 2Wildlife Genomics and Disease Ecology, Department of Veterinary Sciences, University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY 82071, USA 3US Geological Survey, Western Ecological Research Center, Santa Cruz, CA 95060, USA 4Monterey Bay Aquarium, Monterey, CA 93940, USA RBG, 0000-0002-4901-5081; HBE, 0000-0002-0205-8818 Many ecological aspects of tool-use in sea otters are similar to those in Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins. Within an area, most tool-using dolphins share a single mitochondrial haplotype and are more related to each other than to the population as a whole. We asked whether sea otters in California showed similar genetic patterns by sequencing mitogenomes of 43 otters and genotyping 154 otters at 38 microsatellite loci. There were six variable sites in the mitogenome that yielded three haplotypes, one found in only a single individual. The other two haplotypes contained similar percentages (33 and 36%) of frequent tool-users and a variety of diet types. Microsatellite analyses showed that snail specialists, the diet specialist group that most frequently used tools, were no more related to each other than to the popu- lation as a whole. The lack of genetic association among tool-using sea otters compared with dolphins may result from the length of time each species has been using tools. Tool-use in dolphins appears to be a relatively recent inno- vation (less than 200 years) but sea otters have probably been using tools for many thousands or even millions of years.1. Introduction Relatively little is known about tool-use in marine animals: the best-studied species are sea otters (Enhydra lutris) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus), both of which bring tools to the ocean’s surface where they can be easily observed [1]. There are several intriguing parallels between the ecol- ogy of tool-use in California sea otters and dolphins. First, not all individuals in a population use tools [2,3]. Second, tool-use is related to consumption of prey that are difficult to access. Sea otters use rocks or other hard objects to break open well armoured prey such as marine snails [4]. Dolphins use conical sponges as tools to protect their sensitive snouts while probing among rocks for small, burrowing fish that live at the bottom of deep ocean trenches [3,5]. Third, tool-use appears to be a response to resource limitation from high population density, which leads to the development of dietary specialization [6,7]. In California sea otters, food limitation results in individuals in the same area specializing on different prey. Some eat mainly large prey such as abalones and crabs, others mainly urch- ins andmussels, and others mainly small marine snails [7]. Individuals belonging to every diet type sometimes use tools but tool-use is most frequent in those that prey heavily on snails [4]. Tool-use in dolphins occurs in a subset of individuals that specialize on small fish that cannot be accessed without the use of tools and only individuals feeding on these fish use tools [8–10]. Finally, diet prefer- ences appear to be transmitted by mothers to their female offspring in both sea otters [11] and dolphins [12]. Table 1. Mitogenomes in southern sea otters. The three complete mitogenome haplotypes and the four D-loop haplotypes found by Larson et al. [15] are shown for comparison. Individual genotypes are in table S1, electronic supplementary material. snp location: 1491 bp 11679 bp 13547 bp 14101 bp 15365 bp 15592 bp 15593 bp 15609 bp gene: tRNA tRNA-Ser NDS ND6 tRNA-Thr D-loop D-loop D-loop HAPLOTYPE T/C A/G A/G A/G C/T A/G C/T C/T N 1 C A A A C G C T 9 2 C G A A C G C T 1 3 T G G G T G T T 33 A G T C Larson et al. [15] B A T T Larson et al. [15] C G T T Larson et al. [15] D G C C Larson et al. [15] rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org Biol.Lett.13:20160880 2 on March 22, 2017http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from Analyses of maternally inherited mtDNA haplotypes revealed that almost all the tool-using dolphins in an area belong to a single matriline [12,13] and nuclear markers indi- cated that tool-users were more related to other tool-users than expected by chance (although, this findingwas not signifi- cant for the western gulf of Shark Bay). We asked whether sea otters showed similar genetic patterns by analysing mito- chondrial genomes and nuclear microsatellite genotypes of individual sea otters together with behavioural data on their diet and frequency of tool-use.2. Material and methods Individual sea otters were captured and tagged from 2000 to 2014 along the California coast (electronic supplemental material, table 1). We used focal animal sampling [14] to opportunistically record foraging data on individual otters. For each feeding dive, we recorded whether or not prey was captured, prey identifica- tion and the presence or absence of tool-use (see Tinker et al. [7] for detailed methods). Each otter was assigned to one of six diet specialist groups—ABALONE, CRAB, MUSSEL, CLAM, URCHIN or SNAIL—using fractional composition analysis as detailed in Tinker et al. [7]. We excluded abalone captures from analyses of tool-use because we could not consistently determine the frequency of underwater tool-use to obtain abalone [2]. Indi- viduals were considered frequent tool-users if they used tools for at least 40% of observed prey captures, based on a gap in the dis- tribution of tool-use frequency between the individuals that used tools on 1–27% of prey captures and those that used them on 44–90% of prey captures (electronic supplementary material). Dependent pups were not considered in any analyses. We used massively parallel multiplexed sequencing to obtain complete mitochondrial genomes from 43 otters (electronic sup- plementary material) in the hopes of finding new haplotypes, as only three are known from D-loop [15]. We performed a x2 test and a Goodman–Kruskal t test to assess the relationship between the mitochondrial haplotype and diet type. In addition, we geno- typed 38 microsatellite loci from 154 otters. Mitochondrial haplotypes were identified and compared with diet type and whether individuals used tools frequently or infrequently. Micro- satellite genotypes were used to determine if frequent tool-users and otters belonging to the same diet specialist group were more likely to be related to each other than to the population asa whole using a permutation test implemented in the ‘related’ package in R [16] (electronic supplementary material).3. Results Mitochondrial sequencing revealed a 16 431 bp mitogenome with a mean read depth ranging from 21 to 1100. The cytosine homopolymer in the 16 s rRNA starting at position 2644 bp was excluded from analyses because it could not be aligned accurately. Themitogenome contained six variable sites reveal- ing three mitogenome haplotypes (table 1). One haplotype was found in only a single individual. The other two haplo- types contained similar percentages of frequent tool-users (33% in haplotype 1 and 36% in haplotype 3) and there was no significant relationship with diet type (x2 ¼ 18.3, p ¼ 0.405; Goodman–Kruskal t value ¼ 0.055) (table 2). Locations of individual haplotypes are shown in electronic supplemen- tary material, figure S1. We recovered one new D-loop haplotype (GCT) in 23% of the samples (n ¼ 10). The rest of the samples (77%) had D-loop haplotype GTT, congruent with the Larson et al. [15] finding that ‘C’ haplotype for D-loop was most common in California. We did not find their haplotypes ‘A’ or ‘D’, which were less common in their sample (A, 10% and D, 20%), probably by chance. Larson et al. [15] found haplotype ‘B’ only in Alaska. The microsatellite data did not have any geographical structuring (R. B. Gagne 2017, unpublished data). A summary of locus information (e.g. average number of alleles) is included in the electronic supplementary material. Neither the Queller & Goodnight (Q&G) [17] nor the Lynch and Ritland (L&R) [18] estimator found that frequent tool-users (n ¼ 21) were more likely to be related to each other than to the population as a whole (n ¼ 133; L&R expected r ¼ 20.0066, observed r ¼ 20.0067; Q&G r ¼ 20.031, 95% CI 0.021, p. 0.4) (see elec- tronic supplementary material). The Q&G method found no association with any diet type and relatedness, whereas the L&R estimator found the otters specializing upon clams (expected r ¼ 20.0065, observed r ¼ 20.0016, p, 0.05) and crabs (expected r ¼ 20.0066, observed r ¼ 20.0040, p, 0.05) were significantly more likely to be related to those with the same diet type than to those with other diet types (electronic supplementary material, figure S3). However, these values of Table 2. Number of sea otters by diet type and mitogenome haplotype. Number of frequent tool-users (more than or equal to 40% of dives) in parentheses. diet type mitogenome haplotype 1 2 3 total snail 3(3) 0 9(9) 12(12) clam 2 1 4 7 mussel 2 0 3 5 abalone 1 0 1 2 crab 1 0 13(3) 14(3) urchin 0 0 3 3 total 9(3) 1 33(12) 43(15) rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org Biol.Lett.13:20160880 3 on March 22, 2017http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from relatedness are close to zero (i.e. negative and in the third dec- imal place) and can likely be interpreted as zero or no relatedness. On a sample of 11 known mothers and pups, the mean value of the Q&G estimator was closer to the expected value of 0.5 (0.46+0.04) than that of the L&R estimator (0.37+0.03).4. Discussion Despite the many ecological similarities between tool-use in dolphins and sea otters, we found that the genetic patterns in these two species are different. In otters, diet types and tool-use are dispersed across both common mitogenome haplotypes and neither otters that specialize on snails (the diet type that contains most of the frequent tool-users) nor fre- quent tool-users are more related to each other than to the population as a whole. By contrast, tool-use in the dolphins and its associated diet type (small cryptic fish) are predomi- nantly confined to a single mitochondrial matriline in each of the two geographical areas in which it occurs and individuals that use tools are more related to each other than expected (however, relatedness was not statistically significant for the western gulf of Shark Bay) [12,13,19]. The lack of genetic association with tool-use in sea otters, compared with dolphins, may result from the length of time each species has been using tools. Tool-use in dolphins is thought to be a recent innovation [20]; however, it is likely a much older behaviour in sea otters. This is supported by evi- dence that, as in some tool-using birds [21] but unlike dolphins, all young otters appear to be innately predisposed to use tools; orphaned otter pups raised in captivity exhibit rudimentary pounding behaviour without training or previous experience [4,22] and wild pups develop tool-use behaviourbefore weaning, regardless of their mother’s diet type [22]. However, this behaviour only becomes a regular part of adult foraging behaviour under certain ecological conditions [2,4]. Moreover, similar tool-use behaviours occur in all three sea otter sub-species [23], so it is likely that tool-use developed prior to sub-speciation [2]. Conversely, tool-use in dolphins has only been regularly observed within Shark Bay in western Australia. The Miocene ancestors of modern sea otters, Enhy- driodon and Enhydritherium, had already developed large, flat molars suitable for crushing shells and exoskeletons of macro invertebrates [24]. Very hard-shelled prey, such as snails and large clams, are not easily accessed without the use of tools: recent research on sea otter bite-force indicates that the armour- ing of marine snails gives them a hardness that is at the upper limits of what a typical otter can crush with its jaws (C. Law 2017, personal communication). Thus, if the ancestors of modern sea otters were eating hard-shelled molluscs, they were likely using tools to open them. It may be possible not only to confirm that the ancestors of modern sea otters used tools but to estimate the approximate period when this behav- iour emerged if some fossil sea otters exhibit morphological signs of tool-use, such as the depressed sternum seen in some modern individuals [24].Ethics. All fieldwork was conducted with authorization by the US Fish and Wildlife Service under permits issued to M. T. Tinker, and with oversight by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California Santa Cruz. Data accessibility. Mitogenomes for individual otters are in the electronic supplementary material file. Mitogenomes and microsatellite data were submitted to the DRYAD repository with the accession number #(doi:10.5061/dryad.6vf7j, URL:http://datadryad.org/review?doi=- doi:10.5061/dryad.6vf7j) [25]. Field data have been archived in USGS Sciencebase, DOI http://doi.org/10.5066/F78050S9 Authors’ contributions. K.R. conceived the study and wrote the first draft. N.R.M., R.B.G., H.B.E. and J.M. supervised and conducted genetic laboratory work and analyses. J.F. and M.T.T. supervised and con- ducted fieldwork and assigned the otters to diet types. All authors edited and approved the final draft of the manuscript and agree to be accountable for all content. Competing interests. We have no competing interests. Funding. K.R. received Smithsonian Capstone funding. H.B.E. received funding from Monterey Bay Aquarium and the California Depart- ment of Fish and Wildlife. Funding and logistical support for fieldwork were provided by the US Geological Survey, Monterey Bay Aquarium, US Fish and Wildlife, California Coastal Conser- vancy, University of California Natural Reserve System and the California Department of Fish and Wildlife. Acknowledgements. We thank the many dedicated individuals who col- lected the field data and genetic samples. Dave Jessup and Melissa Miller supported the microsatellite project in many ways over a decade, and Lisa Dalbeck, Tracy Drazenovich and Tamala Gilliland assisted with microsatellite genotypes. We thank Palle Villesen and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments. Any use of trade, pro- duct or firm names in this publication is for descriptive purposes only and does not imply endorsement by the US government.References1. Mann J, Patterson EM. 2013 Tool use by aquatic animals. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120424. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0424) 2. Fujii JA, Ralls K, Tinker MT. 2015 Ecological drivers of variation in tool-use frequency across sea otterpopulations. Behav. Ecol. 26, 519–526. (doi:10. 1093/beheco/aru220) 3. Smolker R, Richards A, Connor R, Mann J, Berggren P. 1997 Sponge carrying by dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.): a foraging specialization involving tooluse? Ethology 103, 454–465. (doi:10.1111/j.1439- 0310.1997.tb00160.x) 4. Fujii JA, Ralls K, Tinker MT. In press. Food abundance, prey morphology, and diet specialization influence individual sea otter rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org Biol.Lett.13:20160880 4 on March 22, 2017http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from tool-use. Behav. Ecol. (doi:10.1093/beheco/ arx011) 5. Patterson EM, Mann J. 2011 The ecological conditions that favor tool use and innovation in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.). PLoS ONE 6, e22243. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022243) 6. Svanba¨ck R, Bolnick DI. 2007 Intraspecific competition drives increased resource use diversity within a natural population. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 839–844. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0198) 7. Tinker TM, Guimara˜es PR, Novak M, Marquitti FMD, Bodkin JL, Staedler M, Bentall G, Estes JA. 2012 Structure and mechanism of diet specialisation: testing models of individual variation in resource use with sea otters. Ecol. Lett. 15, 475–483. (doi:10.1111/ j.1461-0248.2012.01760.x) 8. Watson-Capps JJ. 2005 Female mating behavior in the context of sexual coercion and female ranging behavior of bottenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) in Shark Bay, western Australia. PhD Dissertation, Georgetown University. 9. Patterson EM, Krzyszczyk E, Mann J. 2016 Age-specific foraging performance and reproduction in tool-using wild bottlenose dolphins. Behav. Ecol. 27, 401–410. (doi:10. 1093/beheco/arv164) 10. Kru¨tzen M, Kreicker S, MacLeod CD, Learmonth J, Kopps AM, Walsham P, Allen SJ. 2014 Cultural transmission of tool use by Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) provides access to a novel foraging niche. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20140374. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0374)11. Estes J, Riedman M, Staedler M, Tinker M, Lyon B. 2003 Individual variation in prey selection by sea otters: patterns, causes and implications. J. Anim. Ecol. 72, 144–155. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.2003. 00690.x) 12. Kru¨tzen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB. 2005 Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 8939–8943. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 0500232102) 13. Kopps AM, Ackermann CY, Sherwin WB, Allen SJ, Bejder L, Kru¨tzen M. 2014 Cultural transmission of tool use combined with habitat specializations leads to fine-scale genetic structure in bottlenose dolphins. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20133245. (doi:10. 1098/rspb.2013.3245) 14. Altmann J. 1974 Observational study of behavior sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227–267. (doi:10.1163/156853974X00534) 15. Larson S, Jameson R, Etnier M, Fleming M, Bentzen P. 2002 Loss of genetic diversity in sea otters (Enhydra lutris) associated with the fur trade of the 18th and 19th centuries. Mol. Ecol. 11, 1899–1903. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2002.01599.x) 16. Pew J, Muir PH, Wang J, Frasier TR. 2015 related: an R package for analysing pairwise relatedness from codominant molecular markers. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 15, 557–561. (doi:10.1111/1755-0998. 12323) 17. Queller DC, Goodnight KF. 1989 Estimating relatedness using genetic markers. Evolution 43, 258–275. (doi:10.2307/2409206)18. Lynch M, Ritland K. 1999 Estimation of pairwise relatedness with molecular markers. Genetics 152, 1753–1766. 19. Ackermann C. 2008 Contrasting vertical skill transmission patterns of a tool use behaviour in two groups of wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) as revealed by molecular genetic analyses. MA Thesis, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland. 20. Kopps AM, Sherwin WB. 2012 Modelling the emergence and stability of a vertically transmitted cultural trait in bottlenose dolphins. Anim. Behav. 84, 1347–1362. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.029) 21. Rutz C et al. 2016 Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature 537, 403–407. (doi:10.1038/nature19103) 22. Riedman M, Estes JA. 1990 The sea otter (Enhydra lutris): behavior, ecology, and natural history. Biological report (USA). no. 90 (14). 23. Staedler MM. 2011 Maternal care and provisioning in the southern sea otter (Enhydra lutris nereis): reproductive consequences of diet specialization in an apex predator. MA Thesis, University of California, Santa Cruz, USA. 24. Berta A, Morgan GS. 1985 A new sea otter (Carnivora: Mustelidae) from the late Miocene and early Pliocene (Hemphillian) of North America. J. Paleontol. 59, 809–819. 25. Ralls K, Rotzel McInerney N, Gagne RB, Ernest HB, Tinker TM, Fujii J, Maldonado J. 2016 Mitogenomes and relatedness do not predict frequency of tool-use by sea otters. Dryad Digital Repository. (doi:10. 5061/dryad.6vf7j)