ORIGINAL ARTICLE Female parity, male aggression, and the Challenge Hypothesis in wild chimpanzees Marissa E. Sobolewski ? Janine L. Brown ? John C. Mitani Received: 22 March 2012 / Accepted: 24 September 2012  Japan Monkey Centre and Springer Japan 2012 Abstract The Challenge Hypothesis proposes that tes- tosterone mediates aggression during periods of heightened conflict between males, especially episodes that have important fitness consequences. Considerable evidence from seasonally breeding species provides support for this hypothesis, but few data exist in animals that mate year- round. We tested predictions generated by the Challenge Hypothesis in chimpanzees, a non-seasonally breeding primate, through a study of individuals living in an exceptionally large community at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Results indicated that dominance rank had no influence on testosterone levels. Instead of rank influencing testosterone production, additional analyses revealed an important role for reproductive competition. Male chimpanzees displayed more aggression when they were in the same party as parous estrous females than when reproductively active females were unavailable. Male chimpanzees competed more intensely for mating oppor- tunities with parous females than with nulliparas, and as a consequence, males displayed more aggression around the former than the latter. When males accompanied parous estrous females, their urinary testosterone concentrations were significantly higher than baseline concentrations. In contrast, urinary testosterone concentrations did not exceed baseline when males associated with nulliparous estrous females. These differences in testosterone levels could not be attributed to mating per se because males copulated equally often with parous and nulliparous females. Fur- thermore, variation in testosterone concentrations were not due to males gathering together in large parties, as their levels in these situations did not exceed baseline. Taken together, these findings, derived from a relatively large sample of males and estrous females, replicate those from a prior study and furnish additional support for the Challenge Hypothesis. Our results suggest that the Challenge Hypothesis is likely to be broadly applicable to chimpan- zees and increase our understanding of the physiological costs to males who compete for estrous females. Keywords Chimpanzee  Testosterone  Aggression  Challenge Hypothesis Introduction Aggression has been a traditional focus of ethological study (Lorenz 1966), with considerable research devoted to explain its causation, development, function, and evolution (Archer 1988; Nelson 2005). Studies regarding the hor- monal correlates of aggression have featured prominently in discussions of the causal mechanisms underlying the aggressive behavior of vertebrates. While testosterone plays a primary role in reproduction, it has long been known to mediate aggression, as revealed by early studies that showed castration limited the manifestation of aggression in males of several species, including humans (Baum 2002; Soma 2006). Continued research, however, has complicated this picture. While differences between the sexes and between adult and juvenile males furnish additional support for a putative relationship between tes- tosterone and aggression (Nelson 2011), in studies where only intact adult males are considered, a connection M. E. Sobolewski (&)  J. C. Mitani Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan, 1085 South University Avenue, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA e-mail: mesobole@umich.edu J. L. Brown Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, 1500 Remount Road, Front Royal, VA 22630, USA 123 Primates DOI 10.1007/s10329-012-0332-4 between testosterone and aggression does not always exist (Hirschenhauser and Oliveira 2006). The Challenge Hypothesis helps resolve these conflict- ing findings by proposing that testosterone is associated with aggression only in specific social and reproductive contexts (Wingfield et al. 1990). Because chronically ele- vated testosterone levels can have deleterious effects on health, its production is limited to aggression that has particularly important fitness consequences. Aggression associated with competition for mates is a prime example, especially in many seasonally breeding primate species. For example, breeding season testosterone levels of male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) vary positively as a function of how frequently they display aggressive behavior (Higley et al. 1996). These same rhesus males exhibit relatively high levels of aggression while compet- ing for estrous females and display correspondingly high levels of testosterone compared with male muriquis (Brachyteles hypoxanthus) who compete less intensely for mating opportunities (Strier et al. 1999). Additional observations reveal that males in several species of strep- sirrhines and haplorrhines predictably increase their pro- duction of testosterone during the mating season and female conception cycles (strepsirrhines: Cavigelli and Pereira 2000; Fichtel et al. 2007; Gould and Ziegler 2007; Ostner et al. 2008; haplorrhines: Lynch et al. 2002; Bales et al. 2006; Girard-Buttoz et al. 2009; Ostner et al. 2011). The Challenge Hypothesis was originally developed to explain the relationship between male aggression and tes- tosterone secretion in seasonally breeding birds, which display pronounced temporal variation in their aggressive behavior (Wingfield et al. 1987). More evidence is needed to assess its applicability in non-seasonally breeding taxa that lack large seasonal changes in behavior (Archer 2006; Beehner et al. 2009). In this regard, chimpanzees represent a relevant species to test the Challenge Hypothesis as they mate throughout the year and are non-seasonal breeders. Despite the absence of breeding seasons, however, they still provide an opportunity to compare male testosterone concentrations in situations involving intense male repro- ductive aggression and other contexts. This opportunity exists because male competition varies over time and as a function of the individual identity of females (Muller and Wrangham 2004; Muller et al. 2006). Individual females mate during discrete estrous periods where they develop sexual swellings that last about 12?13 days (Furuichi and Hashimoto 2002). Moreover, females give birth only once every 5?6 years (Goodall 1986; Boesch and Boesch-Ach- ermann 2000; Nishida et al. 2003; Sugiyama 2004), leading to an operational sex ratio that is skewed heavily toward males (Emlen and Oring 1977). As a consequence, males compete vigorously to obtain mating and reproductive opportunities with estrous females who are available only rarely (Boesch et al. 2006; Inoue et al. 2008; Wroblewski et al. 2009; Newton-Fisher et al. 2010). Aggression asso- ciated with reproductive competition can be extremely intense, especially for older, parous females, who have already reproduced successfully (Muller et al. 2006). Males compete less intensely for young nulliparous females, who represent less attractive mating partners (ibid.). One previous study has taken advantage of the temporal variation in mating behavior by individual females and differences in male mating preferences to test the Chal- lenge Hypothesis in chimpanzees (Muller and Wrangham 2004). There it was found that male aggression and tes- tosterone levels increased in the presence of parous estrous females. In contrast, aggression and testosterone were not elevated when males were around nulliparous estrous females and when estrous females were absent. Because mating frequencies were the same with both parous and nulliparous females, increases in male testosterone appeared to be related to aggression associated with mate acquisition, rather than the act of mating itself. Finally, a positive relationship between male dominance rank and testosterone concentrations existed, with high-ranking males possessing higher levels than lower ranking indi- viduals during a period of rank stability. While these results support the Challenge Hypothesis, it is unclear whether they can be applied broadly to the behavior of chimpanzees because a relatively small number of females and males were observed. Because variation is a charac- teristic feature of chimpanzee behavior (Wrangham et al. 1994; Boesch et al. 2002), additional data from other chimpanzee communities, including larger samples of males and estrous females, are clearly needed to evaluate the generality of these findings. One possibility not investigated by Muller and Wrang- ham (2004) in their previous study was that high male testosterone levels were a byproduct of large male group size. Large groups of males gather predictably around estrous females (Mitani et al. 2002), and in these, elevated rates of male aggression typically occur (Muller 2002). As a consequence, party size is a likely confound that requires examination and possible control. The presence of alpha males represents another potential confound that warrants investigation because these males are responsible for a disproportionate number of charging displays observed each day (Muller 2002). As a result, male testosterone concentrations might increase simply due to the heightened conflict that surrounds alpha males. Chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda, live in an extremely large community containing over 150 individuals and many adult males and females. The unusually large size of this community creates ample opportunities to observe male?male aggression and mat- ings between males and females that differ in their Primates 123 reproductive states and parity. In this study we extend the findings of previous work by examining whether associa- tions exist between male aggression and testosterone dur- ing interactions with females who differ in parity. Our observations of male chimpanzees, as they compete for matings with multiple parous estrous females over several cycles, provide empirically sufficient samples to test the Challenge Hypothesis in this non-seasonally breeding pri- mate. Specifically, we test the prediction that the presence of parous estrous females leads to heightened rates of male?male aggression, which elevates male chimpanzee testosterone levels above their baseline concentrations. In contrast, we do not expect a similar rise in urinary testos- terone levels in males when they associate with nulliparous females. Although males mate nulliparas, these females do not generate high levels of male competition. Methods Study site and subjects We observed chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. The 30 km2 study area lies at an altitude of about 1,400 m above sea level and consists primarily of mature, evergreen tropical forest (Struhsaker 1997). The Ngogo chimpanzees have been under observation since 1995 (Mitani 2009). As a result, they are well habituated to human presence and can be followed easily and observed closely. The Ngogo chimpanzee community is the largest described in the wild thus far and consisted of approxi- mately 150 individuals at the time of study, including 27 adult males and 19 cycling females. The latter comprised eight parous females and 11 nulliparas. Behavioral observations MES conducted behavioral observations over 14 months during three field seasons, May?July 2006, May?Novem- ber 2007, and February?May 2008. She observed 27 adult males for 1,378 h, recording their identities and noting whether they followed estrous females. For purposes of the following analyses, we defined large groups of males to form on days that the party included more than half of all of the adult males in the community, i.e. C14 males. Although chimpanzees live in fission?fusion communities, whose members split apart and come together throughout the day, at Ngogo, large parties of males are fairly cohesive as they predictably form during periods of high food availability and whenever several females come into estrus simultaneously (Mitani et al. 2002). Male aggression was scored by recording charging dis- plays directed at specific individuals. These displays occur frequently and are particularly conspicuous and easy to observe and record. As a result, they were recorded ad libitum. In prior research, displays have been used to assay male aggression as they correlate positively with male dominance rank, with high-ranking males engaging in this behavior often (Muller 2002). During charges, males become piloerect, shake and break trees and foliage, pull branches in their wake, and run quickly in a straight line through the forest on the ground. Charges typically elicit screams from recipients at whom charges are directed and nearby bystanders; they occasionally result in displaying individuals physically attacking others. We calculated rates of aggression for each male by counting the number of times individuals charged relative to the number of hours they were observed. We combined observations of charges, where the reci- pient was identified, with pant grunts to construct a dom- inance rank matrix. Pant grunts are vocal signals of submission given by low-ranking chimpanzees to high- ranking chimpanzees (Bygott 1979; Hayaki et al. 1989). We ordered individuals from top to bottom so as to mini- mize the number of reversals in the matrix. Only 12 reversals occurred in 872 interactions, producing a linearity index of 98.6 %. Female chimpanzees display sexual swellings when they are in estrus. We considered only those females that exhibited full maximal swellings to be in estrus (Wallis 1992). While observing males during focal sampling ses- sions, MES recorded male copulations with females ad libitum. Copulations were recorded when males mounted estrous females followed by intromission and pelvic thrusting. We counted the number of times males mated females per day to determine male copulation rates. Hormone analyses Urine collection Testosterone was assayed using urine collected non- invasively from male chimpanzee subjects. We analyzed samples from 26 of the 27 adult males (mean = 108 samples/male, SD = 42). Results of assays from one male (pi in Table 1) were inadvertently lost while these data were being transferred to the computer and were thus unavailable for subsequent analysis. Samples were col- lected from males while they were in the same party and associating with estrous females, including those who were parous (mean = 7.5 samples/male, SD = 3.5) and nullip- arous (mean = 6.8 samples/male, SD = 4.3). As noted above, these samples were obtained only when females possessed full, maximal swellings. The eight parous females were observed during 12 cycles (mean = 1.5 cycles/female; SD = 0.8, range 1?3), while the 11 Primates 123 nulliparas were observed during 20 cycles (mean = 1.8 cycles/female; SD = 0.8, range 1?3). We collected 103 samples from 25 males on 25 days when more than half of all of the males were present in the daily party (mean = 4.13 samples/male, SD = 1.9). For these, samples were not collected on days males hunted, patrolled the boundary of their territory, and followed estrous females, as these behaviors are known or hypoth- esized to affect testosterone production (Sobolewski et al. 2012). Finally, the alpha male was not seen on 20 days that we observed parous females. We collected samples from males on these days (mean = 2.4 samples/male, SD = 1.1) to compare with others collected from them when both the alpha male and parous estrous females were present (mean = 6.0 samples/male, SD = 3.3). We also compared the former samples to their baseline levels. Urine was collected from leaf litter on the ground with a pipette. Samples were occasionally caught in a plastic bag when males urinated overhead in trees. Samples cross- contaminated with feces or blood were discarded. Those that were retained were placed in 5 ml tubes and labeled with the male?s name, date, and time. Samples were frozen within 12 h after collection in the field and shipped to the United States on ice using certified transport equipment. All hormone analyses were conducted by MES working under the supervision of JLB at the Smithsonian Conser- vation Biology Institute, Front Royal, Virginia. Testosterone analysis We analyzed testosterone in unprocessed urine using a single antibody enzyme immunoassay (EIA) provided by Coralie Munro from the University of California, Davis (Kersey et al. 2010). Microtitre plates (96 well; Nunc- Immuno, Maxisorp) were coated with a polyclonal testos- terone antiserum (R 156/157; 50 ll per well; diluted 1:7,500 in coating buffer, 0.05 M NaHCO3, pH 9.6) and allowed to set for 12?18 h at 4 C. Unabsorbed antiserum was removed with wash solution (0.149 M NaCl, 0.5 % Tween 20). Testosterone standards (50 ll, range 2.3?600 pg/well, diluted in assay buffer, 0.1 M NaPO4, 0.149 M NaCl, 0.1 % bovine serum albumin, pH 7.0) in triplicate and samples (50 ll) in duplicate were then added to the wells, followed immediately with testosterone-horseradish peroxidase (50 ll, 1:80,000 dilution in assay buffer). Fol- lowing incubation at room temperature for 2 h, plates were washed five times before 100 ll substrate buffer (0.4 mM 2, 20-azino-di-(3-ethylbenzthiazoline sulfonic acid) diam- monium salt, 1.6 mM H2O2, 0.05 M citrate, pH 4.0) was added to each well. After incubation for 30?60 min, the absorbance was measured at 405 nm (540 reference filter) when the optical density in the total binding wells reached *1.0. Intra-assay and inter-assay coefficients of variation (CV) for the internal controls (n = 124 assays) were below 10 and 15 %, with 9.34 % (mean binding, 23.6 %) and 11.89 % (mean binding, 69.5 %) for the high and low samples, respectively, while the CV for the 50 % binding point of the standard curve was 6.36 %. The assay was validated for chimpanzee urine by demonstrating that serial dilutions of pooled urine samples produced displacement curves parallel to those of the testosterone standard curve and that there was significant recovery ([90 %) of exog- enous testosterone added to urine before analysis. We achieved high levels of recovery and accuracy of mea- surement and maintained strict controls for individual variation, sample quality, and assay variance. Creatinine analysis We indexed all urine samples for creatinine (Cr) to account for variations in water content (Taussky 1954). Creatinine is a by-product of muscle breakdown and under normal Table 1 Male dominance rank, age, aggression, and testosterone levels Male Rank Age class Aggression (charges/h) Baseline testosterone (ng/mg Cr) bt 1 Prime 1.04 69 ho 2 Prime 0.83 73 mi 3 Prime 0.97 113 bs 4 Prime 0.55 80 cr 5 Prime 0.58 71 mt 6 Prime 0.28 80 wb 7 Young 0.61 104 lo 8 Prime/old 0.36 99 br 9 Prime 0.38 85 mo 10 Prime/old 0.26 94 hr 11 Prime/old 0.11 88 mw 12 Old 0.13 89 bg 13 Prime 0.14 109 do 14 Old 0.05 77 mg 15 Prime/old 0.18 132 ga 16 Prime 0.57 123 pk 17 Old 0.11 95 or 18 Prime 0.24 119 ro 19 Young 0.29 107 pi 20 Old 0.20 No data bf 21 Old 0.06 77 rh 22 Young 0.29 112 dx 23 Prime 0.04 82 ri 24 Young 0.10 112 ta 25 Young 0.23 111 gz 26 Young 0.12 62 di 27 Prime 0.13 112 Primates 123 conditions is excreted at a constant rate per individual. The creatinine concentrations in urine were determined using a Jaffe reaction. Samples with creatinine concentrations below 0.01 ng/ml were considered too dilute and excluded from analysis; this involved less than 5 % of all samples. Hormone concentrations were divided by creatinine con- centrations and expressed as the concentration of testos- terone (ng)/per mg Cr. Statistical analyses We determined baseline concentrations of testosterone for each male through an iterative process described by Moreira et al. (2001), with minor modifications. This technique has been used successfully in the past to identify baseline values from biologically relevant peaks (Moreira et al. 2001). We began by computing the mean value of all samples for each male. We then removed values that were above 2 standard deviations from the mean. We iterated this procedure until no values outside 2 standard deviations of the mean remained. We employed the resulting mean as the statistical baseline value for that male. We excluded urine samples collected before 0900 h from analyses to control for diurnal variation in testosterone production (Muller and Wrangham 2004). Testosterone varies considerably among individual males (Kempenaers et al. 2008). Therefore, we used each male as his own control for statistical purposes. Specifi- cally, we employed a non-parametric Wilcoxon matched- pairs signed-ranks test to determine if male testosterone concentrations assayed on days when estrous females were present differed from their baseline levels. We also used Wilcoxon tests to investigate whether male aggression and copulation rates varied as a function of female parity. For all of these tests, we separated estrous females into two categories, those who had given birth, i.e. parous females, and those who had not, i.e. nulliparous females. In two additional Wilcoxon tests we examined whether urinary testosterone levels were elevated on days males gathered in large parties compared to their baseline levels and whether male testosterone concentrations differed on days they followed parous estrous females in the presence and absence of the alpha male. Finally, we utilized the mat- ched-pairs design to examine whether male testosterone levels were high on days they followed parous estrous females in the absence of the alpha male. We did so by comparing these to their baseline concentrations. Results Table 1 shows individual males, their dominance ranks, relative ages, aggression rates, and baseline testosterone concentrations. In accord with previous studies (e.g. Muller 2002), high-ranking males at Ngogo displayed higher rates of aggression than did lower ranking males (Spearman r = 0.70, p \ 0.001, N = 27). High-ranking males, how- ever, did not possess high levels of testosterone. There was no relationship between male dominance rank and baseline testosterone levels (Spearman r = 0.34, p [ 0.05, N = 26). To test the Challenge Hypothesis, we examined the relationship between testosterone and aggression as males sought reproductive opportunities with females. Male chimpanzees compete for estrous females, as manifest by their aggressive behavior. Competition, however, varied as a function of female parity. Males displayed more aggression around parous estrous females than they did when no estrous females were present (Wilcoxon test: Z = 4.55, p \ 0.001, N = 27 males; Fig. 1). In contrast, male aggression was not elevated in the presence of nul- liparous estrous females compared to when estrous females were absent (Wilcoxon test: Z = 1.33, p [ 0.15, N = 27 males; Fig. 1). As a consequence, male chimpanzees showed more aggression in the presence of parous estrous females than they did in the presence of nulliparous estrous females (Wilcoxon test: Z = 4.13, p \ 0.001, N = 27 males; Fig. 1). Despite these heightened rates of aggression around parous females, male chimpanzees mated these females as frequently as they did nulliparous females (Wilcoxon test: Z = 1.37, p [ 0.10, N = 27; Fig. 2). The higher rates of aggression stimulated by the pres- ence of parous females in estrus were related to male tes- tosterone levels in predictable ways. Male testosterone was Fig. 1 Male chimpanzee aggression varies as a function of the presence and absence of estrous females. Rates of aggression, assayed by charging displays, when estrous females, either nulliparous or parous individuals, were present are compared with those when estrous females were absent. Displayed are means of individual male means ? 1 SE. N = 27 males. *p \ 0.001 for comparisons between parous estrous females and estrous females absent and between parous estrous females and nulliparous estrous females Primates 123 elevated above baseline levels on days when parous females were present (Wilcoxon test: Z = 4.32, p \ 0.01, N = 26; Fig. 3). In contrast, testosterone concentrations did not differ from baseline on days that males accompa- nied nulliparous females in estrus (Wilcoxon test: Z = 0.70, p [ 0.45, N = 26; Fig. 3). As a result, male urinary testosterone concentrations were also higher when they followed parous estrous females compared to when they accompanied nulliparous estrous females (Wilcoxon test: Z = 3.98, p \ 0.001, N = 26; Fig. 3). The rise in testosterone experienced by males while following parous estrous females could not be attributed to the fact that they formed large parties on these days; male testosterone levels did not exceed baseline on days that they formed large parties in the absence of estrous females (Wilcoxon test: Z = 1.01, p [ 0.30, N = 25). In addition, urinary testosterone concentrations of males who followed parous estrous females did not vary as a function of the presence or absence of the alpha male (Wilcoxon test: Z = 0.03, p [ 0.95, N = 23). Male testosterone levels were also high on days that they followed parous estrous females in the absence of the alpha male; their values on these days consistently exceeded baseline (Wilcoxon test: Z = 2.03, p \ 0.05, N = 24). Discussion Prior research has revealed that dominance rank affects testosterone in male chimpanzees. High-ranking males display relatively high testosterone levels compared with lower ranking individuals (Muehlenbein et al. 2004; Muller and Wrangham 2004). In contrast, there was no relation- ship between these two variables among males in this study. Age is a likely factor that might account for the failure to document a relationship between male rank and testosterone at Ngogo (Table 1). Testosterone decreases with age in human males (Bribiescas 2001), and some high and middle ranking male chimpanzees in our sample were old. Further research will be necessary to investigate this possibility. Additional analyses indicate that male chimpanzees at Ngogo compete for mating opportunities for females, but that they do so primarily for parous females. Male aggression increased on days these females were present compared to days when males followed nulliparous females and on days when estrous females were absent. Testosterone is associated with the heightened aggression over more attractive parous females, as concentrations in the presence of these females were higher than baseline levels and those when males accompanied nulliparous females. The elevation in male testosterone is directly attributable to reproductive competition for females and not associated with increased mating activity per se because males copulated with parous females as often as they did with nulliparous females. Large group size cannot explain these differences because urinary testosterone concentrations did not vary from baseline on days when males formed large parties in the absence of estrous females. In addition, the elevated testosterone levels that we documented in males as they competed for parous estrous females could not be attributed to the presence of the alpha male; male testosterone concentrations did not Fig. 2 Female parity does not affect male chimpanzee copulation rates. Male copulation rates with nulliparous and parous females are shown. Displayed are means of individual male means ? 1 SE. N = 27 males Fig. 3 Male chimpanzee testosterone levels vary as a function of female parity. Male baseline testosterone concentrations are com- pared with their concentrations when they accompanied nulliparous estrous females and parous estrous females. Displayed are means of individual male means ? 1 SE. N = 26 males. *p \ 0.001 for the comparison of male baseline concentrations and those when they followed parous estrous females. **p \ 0.01 for the comparison between nulliparous estrous females and parous estrous females Primates 123 differ in the presence or absence of the alpha, and they continued to exceed baseline when parous estrous females were present and he was not there. Taken together, our results are consistent with the Challenge Hypothesis, which proposes that the production of testosterone will be asso- ciated with aggression directly related to reproduction. The fact that testosterone did not increase in the pres- ence of estrous females, who have not yet given birth, may be surprising. Nulliparas, however, are invariably recent immigrants who experience a prolonged period of infer- tility after moving into their new communities. As a result, these females cycle consistently, sometimes for several years, before giving birth for the first time (Goodall 1986; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann 2000; Nishida et al. 2003; Sugiyama 2004). Because many cycles do not represent legitimate reproductive opportunities, males do not com- pete for nulliparas as vigorously as they do for older, parous females (Muller et al. 2006). Our findings are consistent with the only previous study that has tested the Challenge Hypothesis in wild chim- panzees. In the Kanyawara chimpanzee community, also located in the Kibale National Park, Muller and Wrangham (2004) found that males there selectively increased their rates of aggression in the presence of parous estrous females. Elevated testosterone levels were also displayed by males who accompanied parous estrous females com- pared with those who followed nulliparous estrous females or those who were alone in the absence of any estrous females. Because males failed to mate parous females more frequently than they did nulliparas, Muller and Wrangham (2004) concluded, like us, that the increase in testosterone could be attributed to heightened aggression around parous estrous females in general, rather than mating activity alone. One limitation of this prior study was that data were collected from a relatively small number of males (N = 8?11), nulliparous females (N = 2), and parous females (N = 3), with observations focused on a single parous estrous female during a single estrous cycle. These small samples raise the possibility that the findings of this previous study cannot be generalized to chimpanzees in other communities and as a whole. The results presented here were based on much larger samples of males (N = 26), parous females (N = 8), nulliparous females (N = 11), and reproductive cycles of females (parous females: N = 12; nulliparous: N = 20). Our findings sup- port those derived from studies of other vertebrates (Wingfield 2005) and suggest that the Challenge Hypoth- esis is likely to be broadly applicable to chimpanzees. This study also adds to our understanding of how social interactions influence male testosterone in a species that does not typically display pronounced seasonal variation in aggressive behavior. Most previous tests of the Challenge Hypothesis have been conducted with seasonally breeding taxa that show large fluctuations in aggressive behavior over time (reviews in Oliveira 2004; Hirschenhauser and Oliveira 2006). Few data exist regarding non-seasonally breeding species because such changes in aggression are less obvious, making it difficult to relate changes in tes- tosterone to changes in behavior. The Challenge Hypoth- esis, however, proposes that social interactions that have particularly significant fitness consequences will lead to transient changes in androgen levels irrespective of the time of year, and as a consequence, it should apply to seasonally-breeding and non-seasonally breeding species alike. Because many primates mate year-round, they pro- vide a model taxon to investigate in this regard. Obtaining additional data from these animals represents an important area for future research. Acknowledgments Our fieldwork was sponsored by the Uganda Wildlife Authority, Uganda National Council for Science and Tech- nology, and Makerere University Biological Field Station. We thank A. Magoba, G. Mbabazi, L. Ndangizi, and A. Tumusiime for field assistance, J. Lwanga for logistic aid, and J. Beehner, K. Hosaka, and an anonymous reviewer for comments on the manuscript. N. Presley and S. Putnam provided help in the laboratory with hormone analyses. This research was supported by grants from the L.S.B. Leakey Foundation and the U.S. National Science Foundation (IOB-0516644 and BCS-0752637). This research was conducted in compliance with all legal requirements of Uganda and was approved by the University Committee on the Use and Care of Animals (UCUCA) at the Uni- versity of Michigan (Protocol #1092). References Archer J (1988) The behavioural biology of aggression. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 257 Archer J (2006) Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:319?345 Bales KL, French JA, McWilliams J, Lake RA, Dietz JM (2006) Effects of social status, age, and season on androgen and cortisol levels in wild male golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia). Horm Behav 49:88?95 Baum M (2002) Sex steroids and testosterone. In: Becker J, Breedlove M, Crews D (eds) Behavioral endocrinology, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 120?159 Beehner JC, Gesquiere L, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2009) Testosterone related to age and life-history stages in male baboons and geladas. Horm Behav 56:472?480 Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H (2000) The chimpanzees of the Ta?? forest. Oxford University Press, Oxford, p 316 Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant L (2002) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cam- bridge, p 300 Boesch C, Kohou G, Nene H, Vigilant L (2006) Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Ta?? forest. Am J Phys Anthropol 130:103?115 Bribiescas RG (2001) Reproductive ecology and life history of the human male. Yearb Phys Anthropol 44:148?176 Bygott D (1979) Agonistic behavior, dominance, and social structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg D, McCown E (eds) The great apes. Menlo Park, CA, Benjamin- Cummings, pp 405?427 Primates 123 Cavigelli SA, Pereira ME (2000) Mating season aggression and fecal testosterone levels in male ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Horm Behav 37:246?255 Emlen ST, Oring LW (1977) Ecology, sexual selection, and evolution of mating systems. Science 197:215?223 Fichtel C, Kraus C, Ganswindt A, Heistermann M (2007) Influence of reproductive season and rank on fecal glucocorticoid levels in free-ranging male Verreaux?s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi). Horm Behav 51:640?648 Furuichi T, Hashimoto C (2002) Why female bonobos have a lower copulation rate during estrus than chimpanzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant L (eds) Behavioural diversity in chim- panzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 156?167 Girard-Buttoz C, Heistermann M, Krummel S, Engelhardt A (2009) Seasonal and social influences on fecal androgen and glucocor- ticoid excretion in wild male long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Physiol Behav 98:168?175 Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe. Belknap Press, Cambridge, p 673 Gould L, Ziegler TE (2007) Variation in fecal testosterone levels, inter-male aggression, dominance rank and age during mating and post-mating periods in wild adult male ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Am J Primatol 69:1325?1339 Hayaki H, Huffman M, Nishida T (1989) Dominance among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 30:187?197 Higley JD, King ST, Hasert MF, Champoux M, Suormi SJ, Linnoila M (1996) Stability of inter-individual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to severe aggression and competent social behavior in rhesus macaque females. Neuropsychophar- macology 14:67?76 Hirschenhauser K, Oliveira RF (2006) Social modulation of andro- gens in male vertebrates: meta-analyses of the challenge hypothesis. Anim Behav 71:265?277 Inoue E, Inoue-Murayama M, Vigilant L, Takenaka O, Nishida T (2008) Relatedness in wild chimpanzees: influence of paternity, male philopatry, and demographic factors. Am J Phys Anthropol 137:256?262 Kempenaers B, Peters A, Foerster K (2008) Sources of individual variation in plasma testosterone levels. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:1711?1723 Kersey DC, Wildt DE, Brown JL, Huang Y, Snyder RJ, Monfort SL (2010) Parallel and seasonal changes in gonadal and adrenal hormones in male giant pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca). J Mammal 91:1496?1507 Lorenz K (1966) On aggression. Harcourt, Brace and World, New York, pp 306 Lynch JW, Ziegler TE, Strier KB (2002) Individual and seasonal variation in fecal testosterone and cortisol levels of wild male tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Horm Behav 41:275?287 Mitani JC (2009) Cooperation and competition in chimpanzees: current understanding and future challenges. Evol Anthropol 18:215?227 Mitani J, Watts D, Lwanga J (2002) Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant L (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 102?111 Moreira N, Monteiro-Filho ELA, Moraes W, Swanson WF, Graham LH, Pasquali OL, Gomes MLF, Morais RN, Wildt DE, Brown JL (2001) Reproductive steroid hormones and ovarian activity in felids of the Leopardus genus. Zoo Biol 20:103?116 Muehlenbein MP, Watts DP, Whitten PL (2004) Dominance rank and fecal testosterone levels in adult male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Am J Primatol 64:71?82 Muller M (2002) Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant L (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 112?124 Muller MN, Wrangham RW (2004) Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ?challenge hypothesis?. Anim Behav 67:113?123 Muller MN, Thompson ME, Wrangham RW (2006) Male chimpan- zees prefer mating with old females. Curr Biol 16:2234?2238 Nelson R (2005) Biology of aggression. Oxford University Press, Cary, p 529 Nelson R (2011) An introduction to behavioral endocrinology, 4th edn. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, p 712 Newton-Fisher NE, Thompson ME, Reynolds V, Boesch C, Vigilant L (2010) Paternity and social rank in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) from the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Am J Phys Anthropol 142:417?428 Nishida T, Corp N, Hamai M, Hasegawa T, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M, Hosaka K, Hunt KD, Itoh N, Kawanaka K, Matsumoto-Oda A, Mitani JC, Nakamura M, Norikoshi K, Sakamaki T, Turner L, Uehara S, Zamma K (2003) Demography, female life history, and reproductive profiles among the chimpanzees of Mahale. Am J Primatol 59:99?121 Oliveira RF (2004) Social modulation of androgens in vertebrates: mechanisms and function. Adv Stud Behav 34:165?239 Ostner J, Kappeler PM, Heistermann M (2008) Androgen and glucocorticoid levels reflect seasonally occurring social chal- lenges in male redfronted lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:627?638 Ostner J, Heistermann M, Schulke O (2011) Male competition and its hormonal correlates in Assamese macaques (Macaca assamen- sis). Horm Behav 59:105?113 Sobolewski ME, Brown JL, Mitani JC (2012) Territoriality, tolerance and testosterone in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.018 Soma KK (2006) Testosterone and aggression: Berthold, birds and beyond. J Neuroendocrinol 18:543?551 Strier K, Ziegler T, Wittwer D (1999) Seasonal and social correlates of fecal testosterone and cortisol levels in wild male muriquis (Brachyteles arachnoides). Horm Behav 35:125?134 Struhsaker TT (1997) Ecology of an African rainforest. University Press of Florida, Gainesville, p 432 Sugiyama Y (2004) Demographic parameters and life history of chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Am J Phys Anthropol 124: 154?165 Taussky HH (1954) A microcolorimetric determination of creatinine in urine by the Jaffe reaction. J Biol Chem 208:853?861 Wallis J (1992) Chimpanzee genital swelling and its role in the patterns of sociosexual behavior. Am J Primatol 28:101?113 Wingfield JC (2005) A continuing saga: the role of testosterone in aggression. Horm Behav 48:253?255 Wingfield JC, Ball GF, Dufty AM, Hegner RE, Ramenofsky M (1987) Testosterone and aggression in birds. Am Sci 75:602?608 Wingfield JC, Hegner RE, Dufty AM, Ball GF (1990) The challenge hypothesis?theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies. Am Nat 136:829?846 Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne P (1994) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, p 448 Wroblewski EE, Murray CM, Keele BF, Schumacher-Stankey JC, Hahn BH, Pusey AE (2009) Male dominance rank and repro- ductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Anim Behav 77:873?885 Primates 123