Familiarity breeds progeny: so t m * ian C sonia tate e on contain an adult male year round, although most males live solitarily. We compared Introducti Animals ar of group living exceed costs (Krause and Ruxton 2002). adult females live in groups while adult males typically pper 1996, e Central rican ring- tailed coati (Nasua nasua) social groups typically contain 2009). The presence of adult males in social groups mating season (Kaufman 1962, Booth-Binczik et al. 2004, Hirsch 2007a). If an adult male is already associated with Correspondence: Ben Hirsch; E-mail: hirschb@si.edu Molecular Ecology (2011) 20, 409?419 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2010.04940.xlive alone (Kaufman 1962; Smythe 1970; Russell 1982; Gompper & Krinsley 1992; Gompper 1995). By living alone, adult male coatis increase their foraging success but live under increased predation risk and in some could have important effects on the mating system and distribution of reproduction within and between groups of ring-tailed coatis. In both coati species, adult males violently fight each other for access to receptive females during the shortThese costs and benefits of sociality can differ according to sex, leading to sexual segregation in some species (Conradt 1998; Ruckstuhl & Neuhaus 2002). Coatis (Nasua spp.) show strong patterns of sexual segregation: one adult male throughout the year, while other adult males live alone after dispersing from their natal group at 2 years of age (Alves-Costa et al. 2004; Resende et al. 2004, Hirsch 2007a,b; Costa et al. 2009; Olifiers et al. 2010 Blackand groups of female coatis come into oestrus during the same 1?2 week period. During the mating season, solitary adult males followed groups and fought with the group living male. This aggression was presumably to gain access to receptive females. We expected that high reproductive synchrony would make it difficult or impossible for the one group living male to monopolize and defend the group of oestrous females. However, we found that group living males sired between 67?91% of the offspring in their groups. This reproductive monopolization is much higher than other species of mammals with comparably short mating seasons. Clearly, living in a group greatly enhanced a male?s reproductive success. At the same time, at least 50% of coati litters contained offspring sired by extra-group males (usually only one offspring per litter); thus, resident males could not prevent extra-group matings. The resident male?s reproductive advantage may reflect female preference for a resident male strong enough to fend off competing males. Keywords: coati, extra-pair paternity, mating success, Nasua nasua, paternity, reproductive skew, reproductive synchrony, sociality Received 28 May 2010; revision received 18 October 2010; accepted 21 October 2010 on e predicted to live in groups if the benefits cases have higher ectoparasite loads (Gom 2004; Hass & Valenzuela 2002). Unlike th American species (Nasua narica), South Amedegree to which sociality affects reproductive success. Coati mating is highly seasonal reproductive success of group living and solitary adult male coatis to determine thereproductive success in adul (Nasua nasua) BEN T. HIRSCH*?? and JESUS E. MALDONADO *Center for Conservation and Evolutionary Genetics, Smithson 3001 Connecticut Ave., Washington, DC 20008, USA, ?Smith AA 34002-9898, Barro Colorado Island, Panama, ?New York S Abstract The ring-tailed coati (Nasua nasua) is thwell Publishing Ltdciality increases ale ring-tailed coatis onservation Biology Institute, National Zoological Park, n Tropical Research Institute, Unit 9100 Box 0948, DPO Museum, CEC 3140, Albany, NY 12230, USA ly coati species in which social groups a social group, it may gain priority access to receptive males coexisting in a coati group outside the mating do not exhibit a mating system that exactly replicates 410 B. T . HIRSCH and J . E . MALD ONADOfemales during the mating season. Because groups of adult female coatis are able to exclude individual adult males from dense food patches, females should be able to evict unwanted adult males from their group (Gomp- per 1996; Hirsch 2007b). Despite this ability, females in most ring-tailed coati groups allow a male to enter their group, even when male sociality does not appear to ben- efit the group as a whole. No examples of adult male parental care, such as protection from predators or food provisioning, have been observed in ring-tailed coatis (Di Blanco & Hirsch 2006; Hirsch 2007a,b). It is plausible that male sociality is a function of female choice. If this is true, one could predict that within-group males should have higher reproductive success compared to solitary males. During the short 1?2 week mating season, an influx of solitary male ring-tailed coatis typically enters and follows the social group (Hirsch 2007a). Similar social- mating pattern has been found in some primate species such as patas (Erythrocebus patas) and blue monkeys (Cercopithicus mitus stuhlmanni) (Ohsawa et al. 1993; Cords 2000, 2002; Mugatha et al. 2006). Within-group male coatis often spend several hours per day chasing and fighting; these solitary males and adult males usually lose weight and receive numerous injuries during the mating season (Binzcik 2006; Hirsch 2007a). Because all within-group adult female coatis are simultaneously in oestrus, sequential mate guarding should be difficult or impossible; thus, reproductive skew should be low (Cant 1998; Reeve et al. 1998; Nunn 1999). High reproductive synchrony has been linked to low reproductive skew in multi-male mating systems and to increase extra-pair paternity in single-male mating systems (Stutchbury & Morton 1995; Stutchbury 1998; Westneat & Stewart 2003; Isvaran & Clutton-Brock 2007; Ostner et al. 2008). It has been hypothesized that reproductive synchrony functions to increase female mate choice and reduce the possibility that males can monopolize female reproduction (Emlen & Oring 1977; Stutchbury 1998; Ostner et al. 2008). The social system of ring-tailed coatis differs with respect to many studies of reproductive skew and extra-pair paternity because coatis have one adult male per group and several females. In many mammals, the number of adult males in a group is highly correlated with the number of adult females and their degree of reproductive synchrony (Nunn 1999). Ring-tailed coati groups in Iguazu, Argentina, contain one adult male and 1?10 adult females year round; therefore, there is no possible link between the number of adult males and females in a group during most of the year. In this respect, ring-tailed coatis resemble harem groups (Hec- kel et al. 1999; Pemberton et al. 2002; Fabiani et al. 2004). To our knowledge, no examples of multiple adultthese extensively modelled systems, studies of other species can be used to construct hypotheses concerning reproductive success in ring-tailed coatis. The primary goal of this paper is to test the extent to which three nonmutually exclusive factors determine male repro- ductive success in ring-tailed coatis. 1 Do social males have higher reproductive success than solitary males? If adult male coatis live in social groups year round to increase access to mates during the mating season, the number of offspring sired by social males should be higher than solitary males. 2 Do high levels of reproductive synchrony lead to low reproductive skew? Reproductive synchrony should limit the degree to which a group living male can monopolize mating. Judging by comparable studies of other mammal spe- cies, the ring-tailed coatis? short mating season should prevent group males from siring most of their group?s young (Isvaran & Clutton-Brock 2007; Ostner et al. 2008). To determine predicted values of within-group paternity in ring-tailed coatis, we looked at previous studies of mammal species that live in one-male or harem groups and exhibit short mating seasons (<2 months). Species that formed temporary harem breeding groups had between 40% and 75% within- group paternity (Le Boeuf and Reiter 1988, Hoelzel et al. 1999, Fabiani et al. 2004, JM Pemberton et al. unpublished data cited in Isvaran & Clutton-Brock 2007). In species that form more permanent social groups, within-group males sired between 30 and 50% of offspring (Heckel et al. 1999, Heckel et al. 2003, Ohsawa et al. 2003, Dechmann et al. 2005, Hatcher 2007). We therefore predict that the resident male should sire roughly 30?50% of its group?s offspring. 3 Does the ratio of males to group living females influ- ence reproductive skew? As the number of females in a coati group increases, the number of satellite adult males following the group during the mating season should increase, and the abil- ity of the lone within-group male to defend females and monopolize matings is predicted to decline. Methods The study was conducted in Iguazu National Park, Argentina (54W, 26S), between July 2002 and Decem-season have been reported. When group living males encounter solitary males, they usually fight them off; thus, coati groups may be limited to one adult male because of male?male aggression. Even though coatis 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd ber 2004. A total of 150 coatis were captured in groups and were never observed residing in their natal FA MILIARITY B REEDS PROGENY 41132 ? 10 ? 12 inch Tomahawk or similar traps, immobi- lized with Ketamine and Xylazine and fitted with unique combinations of multicoloured ear tags for indi- vidual identification (Rototag ear tags, Dalton Co.). A small plug of skin tissue was punched out during ear tagging, and the tissue was stored in 10% DMSO saline solution. Samples were kept at room temperature between the date of capture and January 2005. The sam- ples were stored in a )80 C freezer from January 2005 until DNA extraction in August 2007. Genetic sampling focused on four habituated social groups with overlapping home ranges (Hirsch 2007a). Demographic data and group censuses were typically taken at least once a month per group between June 2002 and December 2004. In this population, group sizes ranged from 8 to 65 individuals and group per years included in the paternity analyses ranged from 8 to 54; PQ 2002 = 8, 2003 = 15, GR ?PSG 2002 = 54, PSG 2003 = 12, 2004 = 29, SF 2002 = 25 (Hirsch 2007a,b). In Iguazu, pregnant coatis gave birth to an average of approximately 4.5 offspring per year (range 2?7; Hirsch 2007a). All adult females in the study groups were sex- ually receptive during the mating season and appeared pregnant before the groups disbanded in the nesting season. Differences between the number of mated females and number of females with offspring were typically because of 100% mortality within a litter or the death of the mother. During some years, it was possible to trap and sam- ple every individual in a group, while in other years extensive sampling of some groups was not possible. All adult females and living offspring were captured in a total of five group years (PQ 2002, 2003, PSG 2002, 2003, SF 2002). The juveniles present in these groups were all born in October, but were typically not trapped until January to March of the following year (at 4? 5 months of age); thus, some of the offspring died before we were able to sample them. Late in 2002, five adult females and their juvenile offspring split off from the GR group and formed a new group (PSG group). The 2002 PSG juveniles were sired while the mothers were still part of the larger GR group. During 2004, only 9 of 31 juveniles from the PSG group were cap- tured and sampled. Adult males, adult females and offspring were sam- pled in a total of five group mating seasons (PQ 2003, PSG 2002, 2003, 2004, SF 2002). Some males that were temporarily group members outside of the mating sea- son were also sampled. In most cases, social adult males were not related to any adult females in their social group (B. T. Hirsch and J. E. Maldonado unpub- lished data). Males that were captured and tagged in their natal groups were observed entering neighbouring 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltdgroup at adulthood (Hirsch 2007a). During the mating season (early-mid August), additional satellite adult males were observed following groups. The number of satellite adult males per coati group during the mating season was defined as the total number of adult males seen within 15 m of the social group while adult females were in oestrous. Because we were not able to simultaneously monitor all social groups and it was dif- ficult or impossible to distinguish between unmarked solitary males, the number of males per group recorded during the mating season was likely an underestimate. We often discovered extra-group males when they fought with other adult males; thus, the number of observed males should have correlated closely with the amount of male?male aggression and presumably with the actual number of males attempting to mate. The maximum number of recognizable males observed within a group during the mating season was five males (SF 2003). Solitary males were also seen outside the mating season. Some of these solitary males were trapped and sampled, while others were not. A total of 11 adult males classified as putative fathers were cap- tured and sampled. In some cases (N = 4), males were trapped as subadults (12?23 months old) in their natal group and later observed to be members of another group after they were fully mature males (typically when 3 years of age or older). DNA purification was carried out using a Qiagen Bio- Sprint 96 workstation following the protocol for DNA extraction from animal tissues as supplied by the manu- facturer. All individuals were genotyped at 15 microsat- ellite loci (Ma3, Davis & Strobeck 1998; Pfl2, Pfl8, Pfl9, Kays et al. 2000; PLOT-01, PLOT-04, Fike et al. 2007; PLM12, PLM13, Siripunkaw et al. 2007; F03, H03, E05, H07, A08, F02, D03, Molecular Ecology Resources pri- mer development consortium 2010). The polymerase chain reaction mixtures (25 lL) were composed of 1.5 lL template DNA, 2.5 lL Gold PCR Buffer, 3 lL MgCl2, 2.5 lL dNTP?s, 2 lL BSA, 2 lL Betaine, 1 lL fluorescently labelled forward and reverse primers, 0.15 lL AmpliTaq Gold DNA polymerase (Applied Bio- systems) and 9.35 lL H2O. In cases where reactions did not yield a product, the reaction was repeated with lar- ger quantities of DNA (2?3 lL per reaction). Because of poor amplification, all PLM12 primer reactions con- tained 3 lL of stock DNA. Primers specifically designed for Nasua nasua (F03, H03, E05, H07, A08, F02, D03) with the same annealing temperature were multiplexed in the same PCR using 0.2?0.6 lL of each primer. PCRs began with an extended denaturation of 96 C for 9 min, followed by 95 C denaturation cycle for 45 s, a 45 s annealing cycle and then a 72 C extension cycle for 45 s (annealing temperatures in Table 1). The last Table 1 The 15 microsatellite loci used to determine paternity. A total of 149 individuals were typed. Temp equals annealing tem- pecte lusio Ho 0.55 0.15 0.49 0.58 0.41 0.68 0.71 0.66 0.42 0.51 0.04 0.15 0.71 0.56 0.58 412 B. T . HIRSCH and J . E . MALD ONADOthree 45 s steps were then repeated 34 times, followed by a final 72 C extension cycle of 10 min. For PLM12, we used a modified touchup PCR program. The initial 9-min 96 C denaturation cycle was followed by a 1- min 95 C denaturation cycle, then a 1-min annealing cycle, followed by a 72 C extension cycle for 1 min. The annealing cycle started at 54 C, then was raised one degree each step until the last step reached 57 C. This cycle was repeated 15 times and then ended with perature in the PCR. Hobs and Hexp represent observed and ex berg equilibrium are indicated by asterisks. Locus-specific exc column Temp No. of alleles Size Ma3 58 3 153?157 Pfl2 56 2 148?150 Pfl8 57 5 194?202 Pfl9 53 5 205?217 PLOT-01 64 3 155?159 PLOT-04 64 5 333?347 PLM12 54?57 7 217?229 PLM13 58 3 102?108 F03 60 5 109?119 H03 59 4 114?126 E05 60 2 148?150 H07 59 5 159?173 A08 59 7 213?225 F02 58 2 203?205 D03 58 5 259?269a final 72 C extension cycle of 10 min. Products were electrophoresed through an ABI 3130xl genetic analyzer (Applied Biosystems, Inc., Foster City, CA.). Alleles were sized by comparison with concur- rently run dye-labelled DNA size standards. Fragment size analysis was performed using the GeneMapper software (Applied Biosystems), and each genotype was confirmed by visual inspection of the electrophero- grams. All samples were amplified and genotyped at least two times for each locus. Replicate genotyping was carried out to minimize problems associated with allelic dropout and misclassification of genotypes. If the initial two genotypes derived from a sample did not match, that sample was run two more times and the genotype was determined using a consensus of all four samples. We used the CERVUS 3.0 (Kalinowski et al. 2007) computer program to calculate whether the 15 loci conformed to Hardy?Weinberg equilibrium (Table 1). Two alleles that were not in Hardy?Weinberg equilibrium were included in the analysis because it was determined that their inclusion would not lead to an overestimation of within-group male paternity.Given that most alleles deviate from Hardy?Weinberg because of allelic dropout or null alleles and that these two alleles (Pfl2 and Pfl9) had null frequency values of 0.17 and 0.067, respectively, we believe there were no major issues with including these alleles in the analyses (Dakin & Avise 2004). In addition, there were no allele mismatches at these two loci between known mother? offspring pairs, which is evidence for a low frequency of allelic dropout at these loci. Tests for linkage disequi- d Heterozygosity. Significant deviations from the Hardy?Wein- n probabilities for the second parent are reported in the last He HW Fnull Exclusion 7 0.498 NS )0.058 0.806 4 0.218 ** 0.170 0.903 7 0.492 NS )0.026 0.723 4 0.663 ** 0.067 0.614 6 0.414 NS )0.005 0.833 5 0.698 NS )0.003 0.546 1 0.744 NS 0.023 0.500 4 0.559 NS )0.089 0.705 3 0.396 NS )0.038 0.777 0 0.520 NS 0.014 0.733 0 0.065 NS 0.213 0.969 4 0.145 NS )0.031 0.927 8 0.736 NS 0.017 0.309 4 0.501 NS )0.061 0.813 4 0.587 NS 0.003 0.655librium were implemented in GENEPOP 4.0.10, and no statistically significant evidence for linkage disequilib- rium between pairs of loci was found. We used CERVUS 3.0 to determine paternity and mater- nity assignments of 76 juvenile coatis. Maternity was assigned using a total evidence approach, combining behavioural data (grooming rates between mothers and potential offspring) and genotyping (Prodohl et al. 1998; Slate et al. 2000). In the three cases where geneti- cally assigned maternity did not match the mother pre- dicted by grooming data, we examined LOD scores (combined likelihood ratios of parental assignment) and allele mismatches between the offspring and the pre- dicted mother. In all three cases, the mother predicted using grooming data had a positive LOD score and no allele mismatches; so, we used the behavioural data to determine maternity in these cases, and not the mother assigned by CERVUS. Paternity was calculated using trio LOD scores with known mothers. To simulate the expected probability of correctly determining the father at random from the Ig- uazu population, it was assumed that half of the adult  2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd coatis in the population were sampled. This estimate was derived from the literature on adult sex ratios in Nasua narica, where an average of 5.63 males per social group has been reported (range 2.5?14)(Kaufman 1962; Russell 1979, Gompper et al. 1997; Hass 2002; Hass & Valenzuela 2002; Booth-Binczik et al. 2004; McColgin 2006). Given that a total of four groups and 11 adult males were captured and sampled in Iguazu, a 50% adult male capture rate was determined to be a reason- able estimate. The combined exclusion probability for the second parent was 0.993. Both relaxed and strict trio LOD estimates were calculated based on a simulation of 10 000 offspring with 15 loci and 2.75% mistyped loci (80% critical trio LOD = 0.00, strict 95% critical trio LOD = 2.12). The percentage of mistyped loci entered into the model was based on the observed frequency derived from CERVUS. In cases where the within-group male was known and no father was assigned, it was tiple paternity litters as the dependent variable. Because spring: AK, AE, AM, CU, and c10). If offspring were sired by adult males that were never sampled, using only assigned offspring would lead to an overestimate of within-group paternity (40 of 42 = 95.2% within- group male paternity). If all offspring who were not assigned to the within-group male using the 80% or 95% probability are classified as extra-group offspring, the percentage of within-group paternity ranged between 66.7% and 91.3% using 80% confidence and 44.4?66.7% with 95% confidence. We regard the 95% confidence results as the absolute minimum level of within-group paternity. Because males assigned at the 80% level but not at the 95% level often had zero allele mismatches between father and offspring (N = 5) and had at most one mismatch, we regard the 80% criteria as the most robust measure of paternity for this study. The proportion of within-group paternity (using the 80% assignments) was not correlated with the number ales ing s r gro the es FA MILIARITY B REEDS PROGENY 413the dependent variables were percentages, we logodds transformed the data before running the analyses to better conform to assumptions of normality. Results Likelihood analyses resulted in paternity assignments for 42 of the 74 typed offspring with 95% confidence (56.8% assignment) and 59 of 74 at 80% confidence (79.7% assignment)(Table 2). Of these assigned off- spring, five were assigned to an extra-group male (off- Table 2 The percentage of offspring fathered by within-group m sents the number of adult females in the group during the mat females whose offspring survived the nesting season. Males pe group males observed within 15 m of the social group during sampled for DNA were not included in the table Group Year # Mated $ # Moms # Mal PQ 2003 5 5 1 PSG 2002 9 2 2 PSG 2003 5 3 1 PSG 2004 7 5 3 SF 2002 ?6 6 2 Totalassumed that the offspring was sired by an extra-group male. Original least square (OLS) regressions were cal- culated to determine whether the percentage of within- group paternity per year was related to the number of adult females, number of pregnant females, number of males and male ? female ratio (JMP 5.1.2; SAS Institute). We also ran these analyses with the percentage of mul- 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltdof adult females or adult females with living offspring from that group year (OLS regression: number of females; F1,5 = 0.215, slope = )0.096, P = 0.675, number of mothers; slope = )0.073, F1,5 = 0.118, P = 0.754). Within-group paternity was also not significantly corre- lated with the number of males observed with the group during the mating season or the ratio of observed males to within-group females, but the effect slopes were in the predicted negative direction in both cases (OLS regression: number of males; slope = )0.577, F1,5 = 4.650. P = 0.120, male-female ratio; slope = )5.067, F1,5 = 0.537, P = 0.083). Multiple paternity was found in at least 9 of 17 litters (53%). During some group years, juveniles were trapped several months after the birth season and sev- eral juveniles probably died before being sampled (details in: Hirsch 2007a). If paternity analyses are restricted to 2003, when almost all of the litters were completely sampled, four of eight litters had more than versus extra-group males. The number of mated females repre- eason that could have mated. The number of mothers includes up were calculated based on the number of social and extra- mating season. Juveniles that died before being captured and # Offspring # Fathered within-group % Within-group paternity 95% 80% 95% 80% 23 13 21 0.565 0.913 6 4 5 0.667 0.833 15 9 12 0.600 0.800 9 4 6 0.444 0.667 12 9 9 0.750 0.750 65 39 55 0.600 0.815 Table 3 Paternity assignments for six coati group years. Year indicates the year of birth of the offspring Offspring Mother Pair AM Pair LOD Candidate father Group male Pair AM Pair LOD Trio AM Trio LOD Trio D LOD Within-group male PQ 2002 AA AY 2 )3.961 VI ? 1 )1.245 4 )2.967 5.135 ? AK AY 0 1.661 VI ? 1 )0.444 1 1.810 11.644 No LW AY 0 2.617 VI ? 1 )0.823 2 )2.981 5.801 ? SB AY 1 )0.721 OB1 ? 2 )3.882 3 )3.470 0.417 ? PB GZ 1 )0.843 VI ? 1 )2.090 3 )3.420 5.287 ? PU GZ 0 6.070 TV ? 1 )2.494 2 )2.082 1.089 ? CC MA 0 7.535 TV ? 2 )6.636 2 )4.448 0.112 ? CL MA 0 7.736 VI ? 1 )0.706 1 )2.251 5.369 ? TC MA 0 3.590 EH ? 3 )6.234 4 )8.604 0.406 ? PQ 2003 AD (AN) 0 0.343 OB1 OB1 0 3.876 0 4.264 9.378 *** JK (AN) 0 1.402 OB1 OB1 0 4.602 0 6.367 13.518 *** OV (AN) 0 )0.840 OB1 OB1 0 2.676 1 0.645 1.177 * RR (AN) 0 )0.430 OB1 OB1 0 4.049 1 4.077 11.118 *** DI AY 0 2.163 OB1 OB1 0 2.388 2 0.125 2.118 * GD AY 0 3.290 OB1 OB1 0 2.715 0 5.644 10.815 *** RQ AY 0 1.976 MD OB1 2 )6.194 2 )2.927 1.886 No SV AY 0 4.661 OB1 OB1 0 0.677 1 0.724 3.219 * ED DA 0 2.428 OB1 OB1 0 0.275 0 4.829 9.674 *** OZ DA 0 2.172 OB1 OB1 0 1.551 1 4.218 10.894 *** RY DA 0 1.512 OB1 OB1 1 )1.652 2 0.191 8.705 * SZ DA 0 4.969 OB1 OB1 0 0.275 0 4.876 9.725 *** AL GZ 0 )0.283 OB1 OB1 1 )0.032 2 0.822 13.843 * JF GZ 0 4.483 OB1 OB1 0 )0.282 1 0.689 5.162 * LO GZ 0 )0.226 OB1 OB1 0 1.383 1 4.534 14.645 *** MM GZ 0 1.864 OB1 OB1 0 1.716 1 4.164 11.901 *** OG GZ 0 0.341 OB1 OB1 0 2.898 1 5.234 15.489 *** RX GZ 0 3.108 OB1 OB1 0 1.383 0 6.757 14.460 *** SN GZ 0 3.383 OB1 OB1 0 2.222 1 4.307 7.734 *** AE MA 0 5.022 DaMale OB1 1 )1.745 1 2.242 3.646 No GL MA 0 4.937 OB1 OB1 1 )2.455 1 0.686 5.867 * TL MA 0 5.507 OB1 OB1 0 1.324 0 4.738 11.138 *** VL MA 0 3.586 OB1 OB1 0 3.479 0 7.313 12.640 *** PSG 2002 JS GH 0 4.228 VI VI 0 2.172 0 5.228 13.062 *** SX GH 0 3.369 VI VI 2 )4.760 2 )2.350 2.562 No TM GH 0 3.067 VI VI 0 1.593 0 4.482 12.925 *** BS PS 0 0.879 VI VI 0 1.902 0 5.779 12.097 *** DM PS 0 2.413 VI VI 0 1.848 0 5.239 10.599 *** KG PS 0 2.713 VI VI 1 )1.660 1 1.113 11.009 * PSG 2003 AS CM 0 5.447 VI VI 0 3.045 0 3.833 12.355 *** CV CM 1 0.396 VI VI 1 )0.557 1 3.424 11.121 *** IB CM 0 4.313 VI VI 1 1.461 1 2.552 0.243 *** KH CM 0 4.880 VI VI 0 0.621 1 1.155 0.443 * ZS CM 0 3.674 VI VI 0 3.852 0 6.373 16.337 *** AM GH 0 7.093 VV VI 1 )0.022 1 1.211 5.739 No BJ GH 0 2.818 VI VI 0 0.140 1 2.910 12.320 *** BO GH 0 2.718 VI VI 0 2.245 0 5.570 13.461 *** DH GH 0 3.382 VI VI 0 2.773 0 5.938 16.409 *** ES GH 0 4.319 VI VI 1 )2.761 1 0.599 3.849 * BK NY 0 1.483 VI VI 1 )0.752 1 0.497 3.226 * BM NY 1 1.847 VI VI 1 )0.638 2 )0.476 0.272 No IP NY 0 1.245 OB1 VI 1 0.345 2 )1.866 4.030 No 414 B. T . HIRSCH and J . E . MALD ONADO  2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Table 3 (Continued) 0 1.955 0 2.464 3.790 *** c10 JW 0 6.033 BF VI FA MILIARITY B REEDS PROGENY 415c2 JW 0 6.416 VI VI c5 PS 0 1.532 VI VI c6 PS 0 2.395 MD VI c7 PS 0 0.792 VI VI c3 SX 0 3.160 VI VI c8 SX 0 1.519 VI VI SF 2002 KK BR 0 3.629 WW WW MH BR 0 0.956 WW WW CU EK 0 3.889 IK WW LS EK 1 0.222 MD WW BT LD 0 2.317 WW WW RW MN 0 4.494 MD WW SR MN 1 1.105 WW WW JL MS 0 0.377 WW WW MG SL 0 2.071 WW WW PK SL 0 5.579 WW WW TR SL 0 2.072 WW WWOffspring Mother Pair AM Pair LOD Candidate father Group male RO NY 1 1.830 VI VI RS NY 0 3.811 VI VI PSG 2004 c1 BS 0 2.907 VI VI c9 CM 0 2.521 VI VIone father. In seven cases where multiple paternity was detected, only one offspring in the litter was fathered by a solitary male (Table 3). In one case, two of five off- spring in a litter were sired by extra-group male(s) (female NY during 2003). We were not able to deter- mine whether the same solitary male fathered more than one offspring in the same group or any other group during the same mating season. This might have occurred, but this was not possible to confirm without a complete sampling of potential fathers. There were no significant correlations between group size, the number of females or the male ? female ratio on the percentage of multiple litters in a group (all P values 0.888). The relatively high levels of multiple paternity are evidence that most females were mating with multiple males. Discussion Do social males have higher reproductive success than solitary males? Groups of ring-tailed coatis generally had low levels of extra-group paternity, even though many litters had one offspring fathered by an extra-group male. Within- ZO SL 1 2.694 WW WW AM, number of allelic mismatches. Group males were observed to be ***Strict 95% confidence, *relaxed 80% confidence. ? indicates unknow  2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd0 5.212 0 7.920 9.588 No 0 2.459 0 5.435 16.184 *** 1 )2.757 1 0.189 2.369 * 2 )4.266 2 )3.388 0.178 No 0 1.845 0 5.369 10.813 *** 2 )4.157 2 )3.027 3.990 No 1 )0.918 1 0.473 0.700 * 0 4.046 1 3.928 7.482 *** 0 4.931 1 4.726 7.106 *** 1 )1.813 2 0.181 3.992 No 2 )4.130 3 )3.782 1.652 No 0 6.930 1 5.613 12.078 *** 2 )2.194 4 )7.480 0.492 No 0 3.691 2 3.600 6.641 *** 0 6.579 2 5.005 14.791 *** 0 6.696 1 6.372 8.858 *** 0 2.333 0 4.361 2.352 *** 0 4.457 1 4.166 7.664 ***Pair AM Pair LOD Trio AM Trio LOD Trio D LOD Within-group male 0 0.218 1 2.771 8.009 *** 0 0.894 0 2.772 1.669 *** 0 3.917 0 2.301 1.037 ***group paternity was between 66.7% and 91.3% per group ?year using the relaxed 80% probability. No soli- tary male sampled in this study came close to approaching the reproductive success of the social males. All five offspring assigned to extra-group males at the 80% confidence level were assigned to different fathers (VI, DaMale, VV, IK and BF) and there was no evidence that any one extra-group male sired large numbers of offspring (Table 3). Because of the high degree of breeding synchrony between groups at the study site, it would be difficult or impossible for a soli- tary male to mate with enough females from different groups to achieve similar or greater mating success than social males. This result is strong evidence that social males have higher reproductive success than solitary males. Do high levels of reproductive synchrony lead to low reproductive skew? Levels of mating success for the social males were higher than predicted for a species with high reproduc- tive synchrony and ample opportunities for extra-group copulations (we predicted values ?50%). Two recent 0 2.647 2 2.207 4.538 *** incorporated into the social group during the mating season. n individual. studies using phylogenetic comparisons found that dominant or within-group males must defend more females engaged in extra-group copulations, suggesting average for carnivores (Binzcik 2006; Iossa et al. 2008). 416 B. T . HIRSCH and J . E . MALD ONADOthat the resident male could not prevent such matings. Yet these matings did not lead to high levels of extra- group paternity: did adult females choose to mate with resident males when they were most likely to conceive? Mechanisms leading to high mating success in social male coatis The mechanisms that led to high reproductive success in social male coatis are not clear. Social males may try to outcompete solitary males using sperm competition, which should result in coatis having large testes com- pared to other mammals (Parker et al. 1997; Soulsbury 2010). Even after considering that male coatis exhibit larger testicular volumes during the mating season, the relative testicular volume in coatis is not larger thanthan one female at the same time (but see Wimmer & Kappeler 2002). Current models of reproductive skew cannot explain the reproductive success of group living male ring-tailed coatis. Does the number females and males influence reproductive skew? An increase in the number of females that are simulta- neously in oestrus should lead to a decrease in male monopolization and reproductive skew (Nunn 1999; Isvaran & Clutton-Brock 2007, Ostner et al. 2008). A trend was found that within-group paternity decreased as the number of male to female coatis increased, but this was not statistically significant, which was likely because of the low sample size (n = 5 group years). During the mating season, adult males often engaged in vicious fights for access to adult females. At the end of the mating season, adult males typically had noticeable weight loss and severe cuts and wounds on their body. Even though within-group males sired the majority of offspring, multiple paternity was found in a minimum of 50% of litters. These litters, however, normally con- tained only one offspring of an extra-group male. Manyreproductive skew was lower and extra-group paternity higher in populations with greater reproductive syn- chrony (Isvaran & Clutton-Brock 2007, Ostner et al. 2008). This relationship is consistent with incomplete control models of reproductive skew which predict that a main factor driving reproductive skew is the ability of dominants to monopolize access to females and prevent other males from mating with them (Altmann 1962; Cant 1998; Clutton-Brock 1998; Reeve et al. 1998). Short reproductive seasons generally lead to a high degree of overlap in female oestrous periods, which means thatIn general, coati copulations were not brief and pairs were observed to mate for more than 55 min, which is similar to Nasua narica (Hass & Roback 2000). This behaviour could be related to induced ovulation, although it is not known if coatis are induced or spon- taneous ovulators (Hass & Roback 2000; Lariviere & Ferguson 2002, Lucero et al. 2007; Iossa et al. 2008). These lengthy copulatory bouts likely make sneaky mating more difficult and increase the probability of mating interruption. If within-group males are able to mate with females at will, they should have a lower risk of being interrupted and can copulate with females for the sufficient length of time needed to induce ovula- tion. Alternatively, extra-group males that need to fight the social male for access to females may not be able to mate at will. This hypothesis is complicated by obser- vations that females often leave their group to mate with extra-group males, which has also been observed in N. narica (Hass & Roback 2000; Booth-Binczik et al. 2004). By leaving their group, females are able to mate with extra-group males and lower their risk of being interrupted. If ring-tailed coatis are spontaneous ovula- tors, within-group males may be able to determine the time of ovulation from olfactory cues. Because within- group males have more contact with adult females, it is possible that they have better knowledge about the ideal time for reproduction and are able to outcompete extra-group males using these cues. Alternatively, females may be able to choose when they mate with various males and the high reproductive success of social males could result largely from female mate choice. There are strong reproductive benefits to sociality in male ring-tailed coatis, although the exact mechanisms for this are unclear. It is also uncertain why adult males are found in groups year round. Presumably, adult males can only enter coati groups if females allow it. When the mating season starts, adult females may have already chosen a highly desired adult male to enter their group. The exact traits that are desired or selected for by adult females are unclear, but it appeared that within-group males were larger and better fighters than extra-group males. Because adult males typically fight when they meet, social males may have their competi- tive ability tested several times a year outside the mat- ing season. If resident social males are chiefly responsible for excluding other adult males from social groups, this aggression would limit the number of social males in the population. It is notable that no coati groups have been observed with more than one adult male simultaneously living in the group outside the mating season. It seems plausible that adult male broth- ers or other close kin could form a coalition to enter 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and remain in a social group, but this has never been of ovulation. Social males may be more knowledgeable Lori Eggert and Jenny Fike for primer aliquots. Frank Hailer, Binzcik GA (2006) Reproductive Biology of a Tropical Proyconid, FA MILIARITY B REEDS PROGENY 417Emily Latch and Nancy Rotzel provided BTH valuable advice on primer optimization and laboratory techniques. Mirian Tsuchiya-Jerep graciously provided us with details for the primers she developed and without which this study could not have been done. We are particularly thankful to Mirian and Eduardo Eizirik. BTH thank Charles Janson for his advice and support during the course of the research. This paper benefited tremendously thanks to comments on earlier drafts by Egbert Leigh Jr., Roland Kays, Christie Riehl, Brandt Ryder, Jon Slate and two anonymous reviewers. This study complied with all institutional, national and ASAB ? ABS guidelines for animal welfare. This study was funded by an NSF doctoral dissertation improvement grant (BCS-0314525), the Smithsonian Institution postdoctoral fellowship pro- gramme, the Smithsonian Undersecretary for Science restricted endowment funds, and the Smithsonian Conserva- tion Biology Institute. References Altmann SA (1962) A field study of the sociobiology of the rhesus monkey, Macaca mulatta. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 102, 338?435. Alves-Costa CP, Da Fonseca GAB, Christofaro C (2004) Variation in the diet of the brown nosed coati (Nasua nasua) in Southeastern Brazil. Journal of Mammalogy, 85, 478?482.of group movement patterns and female reproductive status than solitary males. This could give social males an advantage over their competitors. Even though social males have higher reproductive success compared to extra-group males, this sociality comes at a cost. Social adult males are often excluded from defensible food patches (Gompper 1996; Hirsch 2011a,b). Also, the cost of fighting other adult males is visible in the form of numerous injuries and pro- nounced weight loss during the breeding season. Pre- sumably, these costs are outweighed by the increased reproductive success documented by our study, although it is not known whether asocial males have longer lifespans. It is also unclear how and why species level differences in coati male sociality arose. Acknowledgements We thank Yamil Di Blanco, Santiago Escobar, Carolina Ferrari, Mauro Tommone, Fermino Silva and Viviana Mun?oz for help and assistance during the course of the field work. We thankobserved. Adult males are typically extremely aggres- sive towards each other year round and this extreme antagonism may make it impossible for coatis to form multi-male groups. If only the best fighters can be social, sociality may serve as a signal of male fitness. Females may preferentially mate with these well-tested adult males, or mate with social males closer to the time 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltdthe White-nosed Coati. PhD Dissertation, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. Booth-Binczik SD, Binczik GA, Labinsky RF (2004) Lek-like mating in white nosed coatis (Nasua narica): socio-ecological correlates of intraspecific variability in mating systems. Journal of Zoology, 262, 179?185. Cant M (1998) A model for the evolution of reproductive skew without reproductive suppression. Animal Behaviour, 55, 163? 169. Clutton-Brock T (1998) Reproductive skew, concessions and limited control. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 288?292. Conradt L (1998) Could asynchrony in activity between the sexes cause intersexual social segregation in ruminants? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 265, 1359?1363. Cords M (2000) The number of males in guenon groups. In: Primate Males (ed. Kappeler PM), pp. 84?96. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Cords M (2002) When are there influxes in blue monkey groups? In: The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys (eds Glenn ME, Cords M), pp. 189?201. Kluwer Academic ? Plenum Publishers, New York. Costa EMJ, Mauro RA, Silva JSV (2009) Group composition and activity patters of brown-nosed coatis in savanna fragments, Mato Grosso do Sul, Brazil. Brazilian Journal of Biology, 69, 985?991. Dakin EE, Avise JC (2004) Microsatellite null alleles in parentage analysis. Heredity, 93, 504?509. Davis CS, Strobeck C (1998) Isolation, variability, and cross- species amplification of polymorphic microsatellite loci in the family Mustelidae. Molecular Ecology, 7, 1776?1778. Dechmann DKN, Kalko EKV, Konig B, Kerth G (2005) Mating system of a neotropical roost-making bat: the white-throated, round-eared bat, Lophostoma silvicolum (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 58, 316? 325. Di Blanco Y, Hirsch BT (2006) Determinants of vigilance behavior in the ring-tailed coati (Nasua nasua): the importance of within-group spatial position. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 173?182. Emlen ST, Oring LW (1977) Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science, 197, 215?223. Fabiani A, Galimberti G, Sanvito S, Hoelzel AR (2004) Extreme polygyny among southern elephant seals on Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 961?969. Fike JA, Drauch AM, Beasley JC, Dharmarajan G, Rhodes OR (2007) Development of 14 multiplexed microsatellite loci for raccoons Procyon lotor. Molecular Ecology Notes, 7, 525?527. Gompper ME (1995) Nasua narica. Mammalian Species, 487, 1?10. Gompper ME (1996) Sociality and asociality in white-nosed coatis (Nasua narica): foraging costs and benefits. Behavioral Ecology, 7, 254?263. Gompper ME (2004) Correlations of coati (Nasua narica) social structure with parasitism by ticks and chiggers. In: Contri- buciones Mastozoologicas en Homenaje a Bernardo Villa (eds San- chez-Cordero V, Medellin RA), pp. 527?534. Instituto de Biolog??a e Instituto de Ecolog??a, UNAM, Me?xico. Gompper ME, Krinsley JS (1992) Variation in social behavior of adult male coatis (Nasua narica) in Panama. Biotropica, 24, 216?219. Gompper ME, Gittleman JL, Wayne RK (1997) Genetic Molecular Ecology Resources primer development consortium 418 B. T . HIRSCH and J . E . MALD ONADOrelatedness, coalitions, and social behavior of white-nosed coatis (Nasua narica). Animal Behaviour, 53, 781?797. Hass CC (2002) Home-range dynamics of white-nosed coatis in southeastern Arizona. Journal of Mammalogy, 83, 934?946. Hass CC, Roback JF (2000) Copulatory behavior of white-nosed coatis. The Southwestern Naturalist, 45, 329?331. Hass CC, Valenzuela D (2002) Anti-predator benefits of group living in white-nosed coatis (Nasua narica). Behavioral Ecology Sociobiology, 51, 570?578. Hatcher JL (2007) Relating paternity and population genetics in blue monkeys, Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni: empirical results and strategies for obtaining them. MS thesis. Columbia University. Heckel G, Voigt CC, Mayer F, Von Helversen O (1999) Extra- harem paternity in the white-lined bat Saccoptery bileata (Emballonuridae). Behaviour, 186, 1173?1185. Heckel G, Von Helverson O (2003) Genetic mating system and the significance of harem associations in the bat Saccopteryx bilineata. Molecular Ecology, 12, 219?227. Hirsch BT (2007a) Within-group Spatial Position in Ring-Tailed Coatis (Nasua nasua): Balancing Predation, Feeding Success, and Social Competition. PhD Dissertation, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY. Hirsch BT (2007b) Spoiled brats: an extreme form of juvenile dominance in the ring-tailed coati (Nasua nasua). Ethology, 113, 446?456. Hirsch BT (2011a) Spatial position and feeding success in ring- tailed coatis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. doi: 10.1007/ s00265-010-1058-1. Hirsch BT (2011b) Within-group spatial position in ring-tailed coatis: balancing predation, feeding competition, and social competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. doi: 10.1007/ s00265-010-1056-3. Iossa G, Soulsbury CD, Baker PJ, Harris S (2008) Sperm competition and the evolution of testes size in terrestrial mammalian carnivores. Functional Ecology, 22, 655?662. Isvaran K, Clutton-Brock T (2007) Ecological correlates of extragroup paternity in mammals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 274, 219?224. Kalinowski ST, Taper ML, Marshall TC (2007) Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Molecular Ecology, 16, 1099?1106. Kaufman JH (1962) Ecology and the social behavior of the coati, Nasua narica, on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. University of California Publications in Zoology, 60, 95?222. Kays RW, Gittleman JL, Wayne RK (2000) Microsatellite analysis of kinkajou social organization. Molecular Ecology, 7, 743?751. Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, New York, NY. Lariviere S, Ferguson SH (2002) On the evolution of the mammalian baculum: vaginal friction prolonged intromission or induced ovulation? Mammal Review, 32, 283?294. Lucero AL, Morales RH, Mosca S (2007) Detection of estrous cycle of coati Nasua nasua (Carnivora: Procyonidae) in captivity. Biocell, 31, 332. McColgin ME (2006) Sociality and Genetics of a Southeastern Coati (Nasua narica) Population. PhD Dissertation, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN.(2010) Permanent genetic resources added to molecular ecology resources database 1 May 2009?31 July 2009. Molecular Ecology Resources, 9, 1460?1559. Mugatha SM, Ogutu JO, Cords M, Maitima JM (2006) Dynamics of male residence and female oestrus during a breeding season of blue monkeys in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. African Journal of Ecology, 45, 49?54. Nunn CL (1999) The number of males in primate groups: a comparative test of the socioecological model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 46, 1?13. Ohsawa H, Inoue M, Takenaka O (1993) Mating strategy and reproductive success of male patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas). Primates, 34, 533?544. Olifiers N, de Cassia Bianchi R, de Miranda Moura?o G, Gompper ME (2009) Construction of arboreal nests by brown-nosed coatis (Carnivora: Procyonidae: Nasua nasua) in the Brazilian Pantanal. Revista Brasileira de Zoologia, 26, 571? 574. Ostner J, Nunn CL, Schu?lke O (2008) Female reproductive synchrony predicts skewed paternity across primates. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 1150?1158. Parker GA, Ball MA, Stockley P, Gage MJG (1997) Sperm competition games: a prospective analysis of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 264, 1793?1802. Pemberton JM, Albon SD, Guinness FE, Clutton-Brock TH, Dover GA (2002) Behavioral estimates of male mating success tested by DNA fingerprinting in a polygynous mammal. Behavioral Ecology, 3, 66?75. Prodohl PA, Loughry WY, McDonough CM, Nelson WS, Thompson EA, Avise JC (1998) Genetic maternity and paternity in a local population of armadillos assessed by microsatellite DNA markers and field data. American Naturalist, 151, 7?19. Reeve H, Emlen S, Keller L (1998) Reproductive sharing in animal societies: reproductive incentives or incomplete control by dominant breeders? Behavioral Ecology, 9, 267? 278. Resende BD, Mannu M, Izar P, Ottoni EB (2004) Interactions between capuchins and coatis: nonagonistic behaviors and lack of predation. International Journal of Primatology, 25, 1213?1225. Ruckstuhl KE, Neuhaus P (2002) Sexual segregation in ungulates: a comparative test of three hypotheses. Biological Reviews, 77, 77?96. Russell JK (1979) Reciprocity in the Social Behavior of Coatis (Nasua narica). PhD dissertation, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC. Russell JK (1982) Timing of reproduction by coatis (Nasua narica) in relation to fluctuations in food resources. In: The Ecology of a Tropical Forest (eds Leigh EG, Rand AS, Windsor DM), pp. 413?431. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington DC. Siripunkaw CS, Kongrit C, Faries KM, Monello RJ, Gompper ME, Eggert LS (2007) Isolation and characterization of polymorphic microsatellite loci in the raccoon (Procyon lotor). Molecular Ecology Notes, 8, 199?201. Slate J, Marshall T, Pemberton J (2000) A retrospective assessment of the accuracy of the paternity inference program CERVUS. Molecular Ecology, 9, 801?808. 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Smythe N (1970) The adaptive value of social organization of the coati (Nasua narica). Journal of Mammalogy, 51, 818?820. Soulsbury CD (2010) Genetic pattern of paternity and testes size in mammals. PLoS ONE, 5, e9581. Stutchbury BJM (1998) Female mate choice of extra-pair males: breeding synchrony is important. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 43, 213?215. Stutchbury BJM, Morton ES (1995) The effect of breeding synchrony on extra-pair mating systems in songbirds. Behaviour, 132, 675?690. Westneat DF, Stewart IRK (2003) Extra-pair paternity in birds: causes, correlates, and conflict. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics, 34, 365?396. Wimmer B, Kappeler PM (2002) The effects of sexual selection and life history on the genetic structure of redfronted lemur, Eulemur fulvus rufus, groups. Animal Behaviour, 63, 557?568. B.T.H. is a behavioural ecologist who uses mammalian study systems to address questions related to the evolution of social- ity, group formation and geometry, predation, competition, and the ecology of tropical forests. This fieldwork was con- ducted as part of B.T.H.?s Ph.D. project and the genetic ana- lyses were conducted at the Smithsonian Center for Conservation and Evolutionary Genetics during a post-doctoral fellowship. J.E.M. is a research geneticist interested in using molecular genetic tools to answer basic and applied questions dealing with conservation genetics, systematics, behavioural ecology and the evolution and genetic diversity of a variety of mammals. FA MILIARITY B REEDS PROGENY 419 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd