Niche Construction and the Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication BRUCE D. SMITH Many animal species attempt to enhance their environments through niche con- struction or environmental engineering. Such efforts at environmental modification are proposed to play an important and underappreciated role in shaping biotic communities and evolutionary processes.1,2 Homo sapiens is acknowledged as the ultimate niche constructing species in terms of our rich repertoire of ecosys- tem engineering skills and the magnitude of their impact. We have been trying to make the world a better place?for ourselves?for tens of thousands of years. I argue here that it is within this general context of niche-construction behavior that our distant ancestors initially domesticated plants and animals and, in the pro- cess, first gained the ability to significantly alter the world?s environments. The general concept of niche construction also provides the logical link between current efforts to understand domestication being conducted at two disconnected scales of analysis. At the level of individual plant and animal species, on one hand, there recently have been significant advances in our knowledge of the what, when, and where of domestication of an ever-increasing number of species worldwide.3 At the same time, large-scale regional or universal developmental models of the transition to food production continue to be formulated. These incorporate a vari- ety of ??macro-evolutionary?? causal variables that may account for why human societies first domesticated plants and animals.4,5 This essay employs the general concept of niche construction to address the intervening question of how, and to connect these two scales of analysis by identifying the general behavioral context within which human societies responded to ??macroevolutionary?? causal variables and forged new human plant or animal relationships of domestication. REGIONAL AND SPECIES-LEVEL APPROACHES TO UNDERSTAND- ING DOMESTICATION The initial domestication of plants and animals and the subsequent de- velopment of agricultural economies marks a major escalation in the niche- construction potential of human soci- eties. Domesticates provided humans with a significant new means of re- shaping biotic communities and natu- ral landscapes more to their liking. Eight to ten centers of domestication are now recognized worldwide.3?10 These independent centers of domes- tication exhibit considerable variation in terms of climate, physical environ- ment, biotic community composition, and human developmental history. As a result, they comprise a significant comparative data set of independent, parallel, regional-scale case-study sit- uations with which not only to search for similarities, but to consider alter- native explanatory frameworks for domestication. At the same time, dif- ferent plants and animals were inde- pendently domesticated in these world regions, and when taken together, provide an equally diverse set of com- parative case-study opportunities with which to consider domestication at the species level. Efforts to explain and understand domestication carried out at these two different scales of analysis, re- gional and species-specific, also address different aspects of what is increas- ingly recognized as a long, complex, and regionally quite variable co-evo- lutionary process. At the regional level and above, analyses focus on characterization and consideration of the relative importance of a vari- ety of ??macroevolutionary?? variables, among them climate change, popula- tion growth, landscape packing and hardening of between-group bounda- ries, and intra and intergroup com- petition for resources and social sta- tus. Identified as potentially causal in nature, these variables are cast in roles of varying importance in frame- works of explanation that address the central question of why human societies initially established and subsequently sustained relationships of domestication with some species. At the species level of analysis, in contrast, research focuses on docu- menting a variety of different aspects of where and when domestication of different species occurred, and spe- ISSUES Bruce D. Smith is an archeologist inter- ested in the initial domestication of plants and animals worldwide and the associ- ated transition from hunting and gathering to food-production economies. He has recently authored or coauthored articles on this topic in Science, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Trends in Genetics, and Cell, and is co- editor of the recent volume Documenting Domestication: New Genetic and Ar- chaeological Paradigms, University of California Press (2006). A new edition of Smith?s essays on early food production economies in eastern North America, Riv- ers of Change, is being published by the University of Alabama Press (2006). Key words: archeology; agricultural origins; food production; ecosystem engineering; resource management VC 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc. DOI 10.1002/evan.20135 Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). Evolutionary Anthropology 16:188?199 (2007) cifically what genetic and morpholog- ical changes distinguish the newly created domesticates from their wild ancestors. DOCUMENTING THE WHAT, WHEN, AND WHERE OF DOMESTICATION AT THE SPECIES LEVEL Identification of the likely wild ancestors of domesticated species has been accomplished by compar- ing the genetic profile of domesti- cates with that of the modern de- scendant populations of their sus- pected progenitors throughout the present-day range of a species, re- sulting in the identification of the wild populations that provide the closest genetic match.9?14 The pres- ent-day distributions of such ??best- match?? wild populations often can be employed to infer the specific envi- ronmental and spatial contexts in which domestication of the species in question occurred. In a growing number of cases, the earliest directly dated archeological evidence for do- mestication of different species has been recovered from sites located rel- atively close to where the modern best-match wild progenitor popula- tions still exist, providing additional evidence of both the spatial and tem- poral context of initial domestica- tion.15 Not all domesticates, of course, are developed from single progenitor populations. Efforts to unravel the complex hybridiztion histories for a range of domesticated taxa constitute a rapidly expanding area of inquiry.16 The morphological changes that show evidence of domestication in a particular species can also provide insights regarding the specific human behavior patterns that produced them. In seed-bearing plants, for example, a number of distinct morphological markers of domestication, such as seed retention, uniform seed matura- tion, terminal seed clusters, loss of germination dormancy, and increased seed size, have been identified as automatically resulting from a spe- cific set of human behaviors involved in the intervention in the life cycle of the plant populations in question? the deliberate and sustained human planting of stored seed stock.17 Any such morphological changes associated with domestication are, of course, the result of genetic changes in the domesticates. Considerable progress has been made in recent years in gaining a better understand- ing of such changes at the molecular level. More than half a dozen ??do- mestication genes?? have now been identified that control many of the major morphological and other changes associated with domestication in seed-bearing plants. These changes include, for example, seed retention in rice, seed compaction in wheat, loss of branching and germination dormancy in maize, and an increase in fruit size in tomatoes.10,18 Only a few of these genes that have been linked to specific attributes associ- ated with domestication have so far been studied in archeological speci- mens.19,20 Like the morphological changes I have mentioned, genetic changes associated with domestication in a particular species are in turn the result and evidence of a specific set of human behaviors that comprise the established and sustained relation- ships of domestication, such as, for seed plants, the sustained planting and harvesting of stored seed stock. Along with morphological and genetic changes associated with domestica- tion, evidence of the human behavior patterns that comprise a relationship of domestication with a particular species are also sometimes directly observable in the archeological re- cord. Shifts in the age and sex compo- sition of faunal assemblages, for example, can provide a clear signature of initial human herd management and domestication.21 Pollen and phyto- lith evidence of forest clearing, struc- tural evidence of corrals or water man- agement projects such as canals or check dams, and changes in material culture assemblages have also been proposed as potential markers of the existence of relationships of domestica- tion between human societies and a range of different species.17 Paralleling this progress in docu- menting the details of domestication at the species level, general explana- tory approaches focusing on the pos- sible reasons why human societies first domesticated plants and animals also continue to be developed at the regional level or higher. REGIONAL SCALE MACROEVOLUTIONARY CAUSAL EXPLANATIONS FOR WHY HUMAN SOCIETIES DOMESTICATED PLANTS AND ANIMALS In the seven decades since V. Gor- don Childe?s22 pioneering effort to explain the ??Neolithic Revolution?? within a framework of human response to a changing world, researchers have continued to develop a wide range of different overarching ex- planatory frameworks for why humans initially brought a variety of different species under domestication.23 Some of these explanations remain rela- tively simple and straightforward, and are proposed as being univer- sally applicable. In looking outward beyond the actual human behavior patterns that comprised the newly formed relationships of domestica- tion in search of larger-scale causal understanding, for example, recent studies have outlined a universal ex- planation for the transition from hunting and gathering to food pro- duction in terms of end-Pleistocene climatic shifts. It has been argued Along with morphological and genetic changes associated with domestication, evidence of the human behavior patterns that comprise a relationship of domestication with a particular species are also sometimes directly observable in the archeological record. ISSUES Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication 189 that human domestication of plants and animals was not possible before the end of the Pleistocene at about 12,000 B.P. due to several key aspects of Pleistocene climates. Low rainfall over large areas of the globe, as well as low CO2 levels, it is pro- posed, restricted plant growth during the late Pleistocene. Climates are characterized as having been turbu- lent, with high-amplitude fluctua- tions on time scales of less than a decade to a millennium.24,25 With the onset of warmer, wetter climates in the Holocene, however, accompa- nied by an increase in CO2 levels, and most importantly, the establish- ment of much more stable and quies- cent weather patterns, constraints were lifted on human development of viable, sustainable relationships of domestication and agricultural economies. Few researchers would argue with the general point that Pleistocene climates may well have constrained the development of relationships of domestication. Considerably less plausible, however, is the corollary argument that early Holocene climates were the central, universal, and imme- diate cause of plant and animal domestication: that the onset of stable Holocene climates was the ??trigger?? that made subsequent domestication and agriculture not only possible but compulsory, with no alternatives or other options.24,25 Improving Holocene climates, it is proposed, produced greater abundance of plant and animal resources and an associated increase in carrying capacity. Human popula- tion levels, in turn, steadily increased and the ensuing resource competition between neighboring groups generated a ??competitive ratchet?? favoring the or- igin and diffusion of agriculture.25 At the opposite end of the spectrum of proposed universal explanations, Brian Hayden?s26 ??food fight?? theory of domestication assigns a central causal role not to an external environmental variable such as Holocene climatic amelioration, but to an internal social motivating force. According to Hayden, the worldwide domestication of all plant and animal species can be explained in terms of the increasing demand for rare delicacies to enhance competitive feasts hosted by different kin-units within societies in an effort to increase their relative social standing. In stark contrast to such explana- tory frameworks based on ??universal inevitability,?? a rich variety of other far more fine-grained and more empirically based macro-evolution- ary approaches have been proposed at a regional scale of consideration. These regional perspectives incorpo- rate a range of different theoretical perspectives, including human be- havioral ecology, macroevolutionary theory, and several processual approaches.4,5,27?30 These regional- scale models are also region-specific; they focus on the interplay of cli- matic, environmental, and social var- iables within specific natural and cul- tural-developmental contexts. In a landmark study, for example, Flannery31 offers a detailed and well- supported model of cost-based shift- ing resource use in the valley of Oaxaca leading up to the first domes- tication of a crop plant (Cucurbita pepo) anywhere in the Americas. Similarly, Piperno32 addresses the initial domestication of crop plants in the tropical deciduous forests of the Neotropics within an exhaus- tively documented context of post- Pleistocene environmental change and associated shifts in demography and resource selection, arguing that people began to cultivate some plants as soon as the net return from subsistence strategies involving plant propagation exceeded those resulting from full-time foraging. In a compa- rable manner, a developmental sce- nario has been proposed for initial plant domestication in eastern North America that considers the specific attributes of the species in question, as well as their economic roles and habitat preferences in the lead-up to their domestication.33 Various other similar explanatory frameworks, each specifically tai- lored to a particular world region, have been developed in the last dec- ade to address the question of why human societies first domesticated plants and animals, with the Near East drawing the most interest because it provides the most detailed archeological record of the develop- mental transition from hunting and gathering to food production.34?37 In a recent comprehensive overview of alternative macro-evolutionary approaches for the Near East, Zeder5 argues for a nuanced fine-grained framework of explanation incorpo- rating macroevolutionary and proc- essual approaches that also draws on concepts of human agency, his- torical contingency, and directed variation. Although these explanatory frame- works for why human societies established and maintained relation- ships of domestication differ widely in terms of the temporal precedence, causal importance, and interrelated- ness assigned to potential causal var- iables, they all share a common em- phasis on a region-specific approach and the integration of relevant infor- mation of many different kinds. Rather than assessing the relative strengths of these causal explana- tions or adding to them, however, I am interested here in considering how they can be better linked to available evidence regarding the spe- cifics of the what, when, and where of domestication. NICHE CONSTRUCTION: THE GENERAL INTERFACE BETWEEN REGIONAL-SCALE EXPLANATIONS AND SPECIES-LEVEL STUDIES Regional-scale explanatory frame- works that consider the why of domestication continue to be devel- oped and refined for different world areas. Substantial progress continues to be made in documenting the what, when, and where of domestication at the species level. Relatively little attention has been focused, however, on the nature of the interface between these two scales of analysis. How do changes in the various re- gional-scale environmental and cul- tural variables actually influence societies to domesticate some spe- cies? How are such larger-scale ex- planatory frameworks linked, at the species level, to the actual initiation of relationships of domestication by humans? What is the nature of the intersecting general behavioral con- text within which human societies 190 Smith ISSUES establish and sustain relationships of domestication? The term ??niche con- struction?? provides a recently well- characterized and much-discussed heading for consideration of this larger behavioral domain within which human domestication of plants and animals emerged. Niche construction is defined as ??organism-driven environmental mod- ification?? and ??the activities of organ- isms that bring about changes in environment.??1 Not surprisingly, Homo sapiens is acknowledged as being the species with the most impressive re- cord of niche construction. Odling- Smee, Laland, and Feldman1 identify humans as ??the ultimate niche con- structors.?? They also provide abun- dant examples of niche construction by a wide range of other taxa, pro- posing that niche construction plays an important and almost ubiquitous, if underappreciated, general role in shaping environments and evolution- ary processes worldwide. Through niche construction or ecosystem en- gineering many organisms reshape both their own environments and those of other organisms. By forming complex ??engineering webs?? of inter- action, they modify the natural selec- tion pressures acting on a range of other components of the biotic community.2 To have any sort of ev- olutionary effect, however, such niche-construction efforts by humans or other species must persist across generations, either through genes or ??ecological inheritance?? or, in the case of humans, by cultural trans- mission. Human dependence on cul- tural processes, however, does not make human niche construction unique: ??niche construction is a gen- eral process exhibited by all organ- isms??.1 This recognition of the existence of niche construction by other species and across all of the earth?s ecosys- tems and biotic communities pro- vides an important general founda- tion and broad context for under- standing niche construction as carried out by those human popula- tions that initially brought plants and animals under domestication. Much of human niche construction can be included within the general category of ecosystem engineering activities carried out by nonhumans; such activities include tool making, selection and consumption of resour- ces, storage of food and water, farm- ing of food resources, production of detritus, import and export of nutrients, modification of chemical environments, creation of paths, and transport of other organisms.1 Given that niche construction is not a uniquely human attribute, but widely documented among other species, and that most if not all human niche construction efforts can be grouped under general niche construction cat- egories developed for the animal kingdom, it is reasonable to conclude that the human societies involved in the initial domestication of plants and animals worldwide were actively engineering their ecosystems in a va- riety of different ways. Other than cultural inheritance, what qualita- tively distinguishes humans from other species in terms of niche-con- struction efforts is, of course, the di- versity of the human repertoire of ecosystem engineering and the far greater potential impact of human niche-construction efforts. HUMAN NICHE CONSTRUCTION AND DOMESTICATION Against this general backdrop of niche construction as an activity exhibited by all organisms, human efforts to restructure local biotic com- munities have been documented across a wide range of diverse envi- ronmental zones worldwide. Over the years, human efforts at shaping their natural landscapes have been classed under a number of terms, including environmental manipula- tion,38 indigenous management,39 aboriginal agronomy and domestica- tion of environment,40 domesticated landscapes,41,42 indigenous resource management,43 and traditional resource management.44 In addition, general classification schemes for human eco- system engineering have been devel- oped.45,46 All of these roughly synony- mous terms and characterizations fall comfortably under the now far more commonly used and more general heading of niche construction. Both ethnohistorical and present- day descriptions of human niche construction have usually focused on particular types of human resource management influencing wild resour- ces or efforts focused on individual species or species groups of nondo- mesticates. Often, such descriptions are embedded within the context of general descriptions of overall sub- sistence economies of both hunting and gathering and food-producing societies. In contrast to studies that are focused on particular human man- agement activities such as burning, or human intervention in the life cycle of certain wild species, Kat Anderson44 provides comprehensive documentation of the full spectrum of different land management prac- tices employed by Indian societies in what is now California over the past 10,000 years. She includes, for exam- ple, discussions of the following cate- gories of ecosystem engineering that targeted plant populations: burning, coppicing, pruning, sowing, weeding, tilling, selective harvesting, trans- planting, irrigating, harrowing, seed scattering, and digging. As Anderson underscores, all of these different activities comprised an integrated and coordinated traditional resource management strategy of direct and sustained manipulation of a broad array of culturally significant popula- tions of plants and animals and their habitats in order to maintain and heighten their abundance, productiv- ity, and diversity.44 Such broad-spec- trum strategies of human niche con- struction are not unique to Califor- nia. They were and, in some places, still are an integral aspect of the human condition, involving keen ob- servation, patience, experimentation, and long-term relationships with a broad spectrum of plants and ani- mals, and have been adjusted and refined over many generations of ex- perience and contact with the envi- ronment.43,44 Anderson44 defines the traditional resource management strategies of Californian Indian pop- ulations as a ??culturally mediated relationship with the natural world in which humans intervene in the life cycle of native plants and ani- mals to direct their growth and ISSUES Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication 191 reproduction.?? This general defini- tion of human resource management or niche construction, not surpris- ingly, comfortably encompasses most definitions of domestication and sup- ports the recognition of domestica- tion as a particular form of niche construction.17 In contrast to ethnohistorical and present-day research on human re- source management activities, ??the empirical search for signatures of past human cultural niche construc- tion??1 remains a difficult challenge.47 Relatively few archeological indica- tions of human management of non- domesticated plants or animals have been documented to date, and these provide only temporally and geo- graphically scattered clues regarding the time depth and developmental history of integrated systems of human environmental management. Occasionally, however, the archeo- logical evidence of landscape man- agement by human societies can be impressive. In the Phoenix, Tonto, and Tucson Basins of the Southwest United States, for example, more than 550 individual locations where agave plants were transplanted by Hohokam societies at ca. A.D. 600? 1350 have been documented.48?50 Individual agave plants were taken from naturally occurring populations and transplanted to locations near stream or river-valley dwellings and village sites, or into larger fields lo- cated on suboptimal bajadas some distance from dwellings (Fig. 1). Water manipulation features, specifically rainfall runoff collection features and rock-pile complexes around indi- vidual plants to increase soil mois- ture, as well as discarded processing tools, roasting pits, and relict agave populations mark the location of these cultivation sites. In the same general time frame (A.D. 600?1000) human societies in the Owens Valley of eastern Califor- nia were enriching and expanding water-meadow habitats of various wild bulbous hydrophytic plant spe- cies through labor-intensive con- struction of diversion dams and feeder ditches.51,52 These archeologi- cally visible niche-construction activ- ities were sustained into the historic period, when they were described in some detail. The archeological record of eastern North America provides several less compelling but still plausible examples of human management of wild resour- ces. A shift in the relative frequency of gray squirrels versus fox squirrels in Late Archaic period faunal assemb- lages, for example, has been proposed as evidence of the selective cutting of trees in favor of nut- and mast-bearing species.53 In addition, evidence of range extension and a notable increase in the relative abundance of three east- ern North American seed-bearing plants in Late Archaic and Early Wood- land archeobotanical assemblages (ca. 3000?2000 B.P.) has been suggested as indicating that they were cultivated and of considerable economic importance, even though they did not exhibit any unequivocal morphological indications of domestication.33 These three plants were maygrass, Phalaris caroliniana; erect knotweed, Polygonum erectum; and little barley,Hordeum pusillum. The Northwest Coast of North America has also witnessed consider- able recent interest in documentation of various forms of human niche con- struction, both present-day and pre- contact. Studies have included, for example, documentation of human- mediated range extension and trans- Figure 1. Agave management in the Southwest. Grid field features near Stafford Arizona have yielded evidence for the management of transplanted Agave. 192 Smith ISSUES planting of wild root crops, evidence of ??ownership?? and weeding or cultiva- tion of wild plant resources, and con- trolled burning.43,47 One of the most frequently en- countered archeological signatures of environmental management is the deliberate use of fire in controlled sea- sonal burning of vegetation. Such con- trolled burns, while a uniquely human form of niche construction, can also be considered a specific type of conserva- tive, counteractive, stabilizing pertur- bation as carried out by a wide range of other species, in that it involves revers- ing or neutralizing a prior change in the environment in order to maintain a preferred ecosystem state.1 Scheduled burning has been documented histori- cally as an important niche-construc- tion effort employed by human soci- eties in many world areas and has been recognized or suspected in the archeo- logical record of areas as diverse as Australia,40 eastern North America,54 the Neotropics,55 and the west coast of North America.43,44 Controlled burning of vegetation by human societies appears to extend as far back as the be- ginning of the Holocene in the Ameri- cas, perhaps to 30,000 B.P. in Island Southeast Asia,9 and to 55,000 B.P. in South Africa.56 DOMESTICATES PROVIDE EVIDENCE OF NICHE CONSTRUCTION Occasionally, human niche-con- struction efforts in the distant past developed into sustained relation- ships of domestication with particu- lar species. Various aspects of the ar- cheological record of domestication worldwide in fact provide the best in- dication of the existence of compre- hensive human strategies for the overall management of ancient biotic communities. Significantly, and not surprisingly, when considered within the broader context of proposed ubiquitous human niche-construc- tion activities, the development of such relationships of domestication was not limited to a particular place or a narrow time frame. Each of the eight to ten environmentally and cul- turally diverse world regions cur- rently identified as likely independent centers of domestication and agricul- tural origin exhibits a unique multi- ple-millennia sequence of domestica- tion of different species.3?10 Further evidence in support of the proposition that initial domestication occurred within broad strategies of resource management is provided by the diversity of human behavior pat- terns included under the general heading of ??domestication.?? For each of the hundreds of different species of plants and animals brought under domestication at widely scattered times and places around the world, human societies had to develop a unique, species-specific set of new behaviors. As a result, while taxa with similar attributes (for example, annual grasses versus tree crops ver- sus caprines) could be expected to have responded in similar ways to generally similar forms of human intervention in their life cycle, ??domestication?? as a general cate- gory of human activity encompasses a remarkable spectrum of different causal human behavior patterns. A brief consideration of ten of the ear- liest domesticates in different world regions can provide a clear demon- stration of this diversity. The domestic dog (Canis famili- aris) and bottle gourd (Lagenaria siceraria) comprise an unlikely but informative pair in this regard. They are thought to be the first two spe- cies brought under domestication, between 12,000 and 15,000 years ago, somewhere in Asia, and to have likely colonized the Americas in the company of Paleoindian popula- tions.57 Both were primarily of utili- tarian value, dogs for hunting and gourds as containers, rather than food sources. The initial formation of a relationship of domestication with the two has often been charac- terized as not so much involving uni- lateral intervention on the part of humans as it was dogs and bottle gourds being welcomed to colonize and share the human niche.58 Often described as ??camp fol- lowers?? and ??dump heap?? plants, bottle gourds are active pioneers of disturbed soil situations, and do not require much care or attention once introduced into a disturbed human habitat. The initial phase of domesti- cation of the bottle gourd may have been marked by human harvesting of wild gourds and the subsequent dis- card of seeds along with other refuse near habitation sites, establishing them as refuse area colonizers and fellow travelers, with subsequent de- liberate human selection resulting in thicker rind and larger fruits. Human refuse may also have played a role in initially attracting dogs to come into closer contact with humans. Movement by canines into the ??human niche?? can also be associated with their automatic self- selection for reduced aggression, thereby facilitating the development of a relationship of domestication. Reduced aggression in dogs as they occupy the human niche is reflected in linked morphological markers such as snout shortening, as well as crowding and size reduction in teeth.59,60 As was the case with the dog and bottle gourd, the initial domestica- tion of the fig (Ficus carica) in the Near East by 11,400 years ago also involved a relatively limited level of intervention by human societies in the life cycle of the species in ques- tion: the simple cutting and planting of branches from fig trees.61 This simple human action, however, reflected a sophisticated biological knowledge of their environment, since the fig trees that were selected for such veg- etative cloning were parthenocarpic, For each of the hundreds of different species of plants and animals brought under domestication at widely scattered times and places around the world, human societies had to develop a unique, species-specific set of new behaviors. ISSUES Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication 193 and while they produced soft sweet fruit, they did not set germinative seeds. As a result, they would have been reproductive dead-ends unless humans intervened by deliberately propagating their shoots.61?63 As was the case with agave, such replanting of fig branches could also have taken place close to human habitation sites, expanding the niche of the en- couraged species while making each year?s fig crop easier to monitor and harvest. In addition, since such planted shoots could not be expected to yield fruit for some years, humans had clearly accepted the delayed re- ward associated with a slow-matur- ing crop. Like the fig, the banana Musa sp., another relatively long-lived and delayed-yield tree crop, was the sub- ject of early human management as part an overall strategy of landscape modification, but with a substan- tially greater level of human invest- ment of labor. The archeological site of Kuk Swamp, situated at an eleva- tion of 1,560 meters above sea level in highland New Guinea, has yielded Musa phytoliths thought to reflect experimental cultivation of indige- nous plants by 10,000 B.P., followed by the construction of increasingly substantial water management canals associated with the cultiva- tion of bananas and other crops af- ter 7,000 B.P.9 Kuk Swamp, in island Southeast Asia, thus provides very early evidence of the apparent relocation and transplanting of a tree crop outside of its natural range, along with substantial human investment in sustained manage- ment paired with a clear awareness of delayed return. Although the goat (Capra hircus) is far different from figs and bananas in most respects, and required quite different efforts on the part of humans in the initial creation of a relationship of domestication, it again reflects both a sophisticated human understanding of its manage- ment requirements and the acknowl- edgment of delayed return linked to a substantial commitment to sus- tained management. By 10,000 years ago, human societies in the Zagros Mountains of present-day Iran had intervened to a significant extent in the life cycle of goat herds, and had taken control of their reproduction. As is the case with other domesti- cated livestock species worldwide, humans selectively culled immature males, restructuring breeding popu- lations into the distinctive general age and sex profile that is the hall- mark of present-day livestock man- agement for meat production: a sub- stantial majority of adult breeding females are kept along with a few adult males.21 In the Americas, two species of squash provide the earliest evidence of domestication of food crops. In Mexico, an increase in seed size marks the initial domestication of Cucurbita pepo at ca. 10,000 B.P.,64,65 while in Ecuador, at about the same time, an increase in the size of rind phytoliths, reflecting an increase in fruit size, indicates the deliberate human cultivation of Cucurbita ecua- dorensis.66 As is the case for seed- bearing plants worldwide, among them barley, maize, millets, rice, sor- ghum sunflower, and wheat, the spe- cific set of human behaviors involved in the initial domestication of these two squashes centered on the sus- tained planting of stored seed stock in prepared planting areas. Plants with starch-rich under- ground organs, such as manioc (Manihot esculenta), arrowroot (Mar- anta arundinacea), and leren (Cala- thea allouia), were also domesticated very early in South America, based on the recovery of diagnostic root crop starch grains and phytoliths from 9,000 to 8,000 B.P. con- texts.32,67 As with other species in the general category of root crops, initial human cultivation of manioc, leren, and arrowroot would have involved the replanting of clonal fragments of parent plants in concert with sustained cultivation and delib- erate selection for desired attributes such as larger tubers and preferred starch types. Interestingly, at the same time that recent research in different world regions has documented an increas- ingly diverse range of human behav- ior patterns associated with success- ful early domestication of plants and animals, there has also been substan- tial recent evidence of early efforts that were not sustained over the long term. Initial efforts to domesticate barley (Hordeum spontaneum), oats (Avena sterilis), and rye (Secale cerale), for example, dating from before 11,000 B.P. (and the end- Pleistocene climatic shift?) have been recognized in the Near East.68 Each of these early efforts at plant man- agement and manipulation was abandoned, however. Sustained culti- vation of the species in question did not occur until several thousand years later in different locales. In the Americas a species of runner bean (Phaseolus sp.) has similarly been identified as a possible early, ca. 9,000 B.P., subject of human efforts at cultivation and management that were not sustained.32 It has also recently been suggested that the sun- flower (Helianthus annuus) may have been domesticated twice, with the domestication of wild populations in eastern North America at ca 4,400 B.P. being sustained and leading to all documented present-day cultivar varieties; a second, roughly contem- poraneous and independent domesti- cation of the sunflower in Mexico was subsequently abandoned.33 In eastern North America, three indige- nous seed plants, maygrass (Phalaris caroliniana), erect knotweed (Poly- gonum erectum), and little barley (Hordeum pusillum), have also been identified as likely having been culti- vated and of considerable economic importance between 3,000 and 2,000 B.P., even though they do not exhibit any unequivocal morphological indi- cations of domestication and subse- quently disappeared from cultiva- tion.33 This brief consideration of the diverse array of human activities involved in the initial domestication of plants and animals, both sus- tained and short-lived, when com- bined with other lines of evidence discussed, provides considerable sup- port for the proposition that plants and animals were initially domesti- cated within a broad behavioral con- text of human niche construction. What then, does placing human domestication within such a broad behavioral context add to our overall understanding of this major transi- tion in human history? 194 Smith ISSUES DISCUSSION Often described under a range of different terms, including environ- mental manipulation, indigenous management, domestication of the environment, and traditional re- source management, niche construc- tion is the broad strategy employed by humans to shape, enhance, and sustain their ??natural?? world while also expanding their developmental horizons. More than 40 years ago, James Downs27 noted the substantial variation that existed among differ- ent small kin groups of Great Basin hunter-gatherers in the details of how they managed and manipulated their environment. He identified two factors, variation and manipulation, as central to the general evolutionary potential of human societies. More recently, Odling-Smee, Laland, and Feldman1 have argued that environ- mental engineering or niche con- struction is a major, if widely under- appreciated general factor in the overall shaping of biotic commun- ities and evolutionary processes. Here I argue that niche construction provides an important evolutionary and behavioral context for under- standing one of the major transfor- mations in the history of our species, the initial domestication of plants and animals. As outlined earlier, observations and arguments in support of the proposition that the human societies that initially domesticated plants and animals worldwide did so within a preexisting broad context of environ- mental engineering can be organized under at least eight different catego- ries of general observations and arguments. First, niche construction by a large number of other animal species has been documented across all of the earth?s ecosystems and bi- otic communities. Given that so many different animal species manipulate their environments, it is reasonable to assume that human populations have been actively man- aging environments to varying degrees for tens of thousands of years. Second, It is also reasonable to assume that such past patterns of human resource management were both sophisticated and constantly being refined. Humans, after all, are acknowledged to be the ultimate niche constructors, both in terms of the diversity of different ways in which we manipulate the world around us and the magnitude of our resultant impacts. Third, ethohistori- cal and present-day studies have documented a growing inventory of the different ways in which human societies actively intervene in their local environments in an effort to shape them more to their liking. Fourth, recent research is beginning to provide much more fine-grained and comprehensive understanding of the overall coordinated management strategies employed by human soci- eties, strategies that embrace many different forms of environmental manipulation and involve a variety of different organic and inorganic eco- system components. Fifth, archeolo- gists and archeobiologists continue to look for and find increasing evi- dence of a range of forms of human niche construction in the archeologi- cal record, extending across many of the world?s terrestrial ecosystems. Sixth, the widely dispersed and iso- lated record of domestication of dif- ferent species of plants and animals, globally scattered in time and space, suggests the existence of an underly- ing shared general pattern of human behavior. Seventh, domestication of hundreds of species, with different species-specific management or ma- nipulation challenges, called for a wide variety of human behavior sets. Eighth, increasing documentation of multiple efforts to establish relation- ships of domestication, which some- times were not sustained, under- scores the extent to which human societies, with a diverse array of skills, were consistently experiment- ing across a broad range of species in an effort to enhance their environ- ment. Based on these supporting arguments and observations, a plau- sible case can be presented that do- mestication quite likely occurred within integrated strategies of eco- system engineering based on a com- prehensive storehouse of knowledge about local biotic communities that had been acquired over hundreds, if not thousands, of years of direct ex- perience.43,44 How then, does this recognition that relationships of domestication were forged within a broad behavio- rial context of coherent environmen- tal management strategies add to our understanding of this major develop- mental episode? First, and perhaps most importantly, by situating do- mestication within a broad and well- documented category of general human behavior we remove, to a considerable extent, the proximate mystery surrounding exactly how domestication was accomplished. Whatever the exact nature and role of the particular macroevolutionary causal factors that may have been in play in independent centers of domes- tication, human societies responded not in terms of spontaneous and anomalous acts of isolated inventive genius, but rather within a coherent and broadly based approach to man- aging and enhancing their environ- ment. Domestication was not the product of unusual ??outside the en- velope?? behavior patterns, but em- Whatever the exact nature and role of the particular macroevolutionary causal factors that may have been in play in independent centers of domestication, human societies responded not in terms of spontaneous and anomalous acts of isolated inventive genius, but rather within a coherent and broadly based approach to managing and enhancing their environment. ISSUES Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication 195 erged out of coherent preexisting resource management systems. Mac- roevolutionary forces did not directly cause domestication, but rather resulted in human societies intensify- ing their niche-construction efforts. The human societies that initially domesticated different species in dif- ferent world regions, I argue, were involved in the active, ongoing manipulation of many species in a variety of ways. As macroevolution- ary variables caused overall expan- sion and intensification of efforts at resource manipulation, a few of the varied efforts at manipulation of dif- ferent plants and animals produced an advantageous enhancement of abundance and productivity far be- yond what was achieved with the vast majority of other species being managed. Niche construction thus provides a general universal context for domestication that is, in fact, rel- evant across a wide range of differ- ent environmental zones and within a broadly diverse set of cultural de- velopmental trajectories. Even though human societies in very different ecosystem settings, from south China to the highlands of South America, independently domesticated a wide range of different species of plants and animals at different times and in different sequences, they all share a common behavioral strategy of niche construction. Recognition of this broad context of human ecosystem engi- neering also underscores the general manner in which human societies identified and targeted certain spe- cies and species groups for further intervention and manipulation and, in the process, highlights several interesting related issues that may represent promising future areas of research. When initially established, for example, relationships of domestica- tion cannot be assumed to have dif- fered from other forms of niche con- struction either qualitatively, for example in terms of inherent intel- lectual complexity, or in terms of required human labor investment. The specific human behavior pat- terns that comprised relationships of domestication with managed species were not necessarily either more challenging or more labor intensive initially than were other niche-con- struction activities carried out by human populations. Within such long-term sustained strategies of eco- system engineering, human societies carried out ??trial and error,??1 ??tin- kering,??18 or ??experimentation??31 that involved repeated auditioning of a wide range of species with a constant stream of different forms of manage- ment in an effort to identify new and better ways of shaping and enhanc- ing their niche. Most such ecosystem engineering by human societies, and most species they manipulated, had inherent limitations, and did not hold the promise of open-ended expansion or lead to impressive returns, even though they were inte- gral components of successful, sus- tainable, long-term human adapta- tions to local environments. Placing those particular behavior patterns that produced domesticates within the broader comparative context pro- vided by other parallel human efforts at landscape management may offer clues to other relevant aspects of the process by which humans shifted to a greater economic reliance on domes- ticates. Ongoing consideration of why some species went on to have long and illustrious careers as domesti- cates while others did not3,4,6,33 holds the promise of continuing to provide substantial new insights regarding the particular attributes and profiles of preadaptation that humans were able to recognize during broad-scale niche-construction auditioning for potential star performers. Situated at this interface of macro- evolutionary causation on the one side and the formation of a multi- tude of species-specific relationships of domestication on the other, human niche construction also pro- vides a vantage point for speculation on two other major and widely rec- ognized aspects of the initial domes- tication of plants and animals: the extent to which the ??broad-scale rev- olution?? may reflect intensified resource management by human societies and the potential applica- tion of niche-construction theory in gaining a better understanding of the apparent ??rich resource zone?? con- text of domestication of plants and animals worldwide. Establishing the structure, com- plexity, and impact of human niche construction strategies back into the Pleistocene remains a challenging and potentially rewarding area of in- quiry. The very early domestication of utilitarian species such as the dog and bottle gourd, along with tanta- lizing evidence of possible deliberate burning of vegetation, provide most of the admittedly limited direct evi- dence that human societies were active ecosystem engineers well back into the Pleistocene and likely had the potential for establishing rela- tionships of domestication. Interestingly, both the dog and bottle gourd fall toward the lower end of the scale of required human intervention in establishing and sus- taining such new relationships. As the search for evidence of niche con- struction is extended back into the Pleistocene in a more concerted manner, it will be interesting to see to what extent the ??broad spectrum revolution??69 may be associated with intensification of human efforts at resource management. Reflecting a dramatic increase in the range of species being exploited by human societies and documented in many world regions as overlapping with and immediately preceding domesti- cation, the broad-scale revolution is often identified as evidence of a growing human population, with resource imbalance that necessitated increased human reliance on higher cost, lower yield resources.37 This dramatic increase in the number and diversity of species, whatever its cause, may also reflect an associated expansion and intensification of human niche construction activities. At the same time that human soci- eties were working their way down the preferred species list and increas- ing their diversity index of exploited species, they could well have also been expanding and intensifying their ecosystem engineering efforts. It will be interesting to see to what extent new markers of human resource management may be hiding in plain sight within the increasingly well documented developmental pat- terns of the broad-scale revolution. Niche-construction theory also provides an interesting new perspec- 196 Smith ISSUES tive on why a variety of the world?s first food crops appear to have been domesticated in relatively similar habitats. Species that were initially domesticated for their food value, rather than utilitarian uses, and which involved more capital invest- ment in terms of human effort in intervention, first appear as domesti- cates in very similar environmental settings in many of the independent centers of domestication so far iden- tified worldwide. In the Yangtze River Valley corridor in China, for example, as well as in the Near East, Sub-Saharan Africa, eastern North America, and Mexico, very early evi- dence domestication of a range of different species comes from settle- ments situated in very rich resource zones associated with river valleys, lake margins, and springs.6,33,70 In none of these areas did relationships of domestication appear to have de- veloped within a ??necessity is the mother of invention?? context as pop- ulation growth forced humans into marginal environmental zones.69,70 Rather, these relationships developed within rich resource situations that enabled expanded and sustained human experimental intervention in the life cycle of a broad spectrum of different species and the adoption of a sedentary, logistically based economy. This rich environmental context for initial human domestication of plants and animals fits comfortably within the expectations of niche-con- struction theory.1 Local habitat set- tings that were rich in biotic resour- ces (species abundance and diversity, as well as species with high biotic potential) would have provided the greatest opportunity for human societies to expand and enrich their overall integrated resource-manage- ment strategies. The greater the range of species included in human efforts at intervention auditions, and the wider the range of different potential forms of intervention that could be attempted, the greater the likelihood that relationships of do- mestication would have been suc- cessful and sustained. According to niche-construction theory, within the context of post-Pleis- tocene climatic improvement and the associated increase in resource gra- dients within spatially heterogeneous environments (increasingly nonuni- form distribution of resources), the richer resource zones, those that exhibited a greater capacity for sup- porting more people in more perma- nent settlements, could be expected to have witnessed stronger sustained niche-construction efforts. Trajecto- ries of change would have been ??chan- neled toward adaptation to those regions of niche space in which abun- dance is greatest, rather than to other regions.??1 Similarly, with increased sedentism and sustained niche con- struction over many generations, the impact and potency of human ecosys- tem engineering, with the sheer per- sistent repetition of the same niche- construction activities, ??might act like a persistent unidirectional ?pump,???1 directing trajectories of human devel- opment toward greater investment in and dependence on intensified niche- construction activities. While it is, of course, dangerous to place too much reliance on ??unidirectional pumps?? and ??channeled?? adaptation, any more than on climatic ??triggers,?? niche-construction theory can be con- sidered to provide additional concep- tual insight regarding why domestica- tion appears to have occurred within rich resource zones in many different world regions. The apparent shared environmen- tal setting of initial domestication of food crops across a wide range of ecosystems worldwide provides a third common aspect of this major developmental shift in human his- tory. Human domestication of crop plants and livestock species appears to have occurred in many different world regions, in resource-rich envi- ronmental zones, within a broad general behavioral context of niche- construction strategies and nested in an associated broad-scale revolution in subsistence economies. In conclusion, while not meant to represent any sort of overarching ex- planatory framework for domestica- tion, these proposed common as- pects of human domestication of plants and animals worldwide, when taken together, do provide new ways of looking at one part of this com- plex process: the environmental and behavioral interface between macroe- volutional causal factors and the establishment of new relationships of domestication. This discussion also helps focus attention on many cen- tral and still-open questions regard- ing the relative importance and ori- gin of environmental or cultural cir- cumscription in the initial domestication of plants and animals. The resource- rich river valleys and lake or spring margins that witnessed much of the early domestication of plants and animals existed within regional mo- saics of habitats of varying biotic abundance, with environmental gra- dients forming natural clinal boun- daries. Within the rich resource- zone pieces of the mosaic that could support greater residential stability and ??logistical?? subsistence strat- egies, it is still difficult to establish exactly why stronger and less per- meable cultural territorial bounda- ries may have been established around the resource catchment areas of different communities. Was the apparent formation of strength- ened and sustained cultural bounda- ries in such resource-rich zones the result of population growth, peer polity packing, and intergroup com- petition for resources? Or is it possi- ble that such ??affluent?? and ??logisti- cally?? organized communities more firmly defined their own territorial boundaries as an aspect of an ongoing process of investment in, enhancement of, and conceptual attachment to an ever-improving anthropogenic landscape? REFERENCES 1 Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman W. 2003. Niche construction. Monographs in Popu- lation Biology 37. Princeton: Princeton Univer- sity Press. 2 Jones CG, Lawton JH, Shachak M. 1997. Pos- itive and negative effects of organisms as physi- cal ecosystem engineers. Ecology 78:1946? 1957. 3 Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, editors. 2006. Documenting domestication: new genetic and archaeological paradigms. Berkeley: University of California Press. 4 Zeder MA. 2006. Central questions in the domestication of plants and animals. Evol An- thropol 15:105?117. 5 Zeder MA. 2007. The Neolithic macro-(r)evo- lution: macroevolutionary theory and the study of culture change. J Archaeol Res. In press. 6 Smith BD. 1998. The emergence of agricul- ture. New York: W.H. Freeman. ISSUES Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication 197 7 Smith BD. 2006. Eastern North America as an independent center of plant domestication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1223?1228. 8 Piperno DR, Pearsall D. 1998. The origins of agriculture in the lowland Neotropics. New York: Academic Press. 9 Denham T, Barton H. 2006. The emergence of agriculture in New Guinea. In: Kennett D, Winterhalder B, editors. Behavioral ecology and the transition to agriculture. Berkeley: Univer- sity of California Press. p 237?265. 10 Doebley J, Gaut BS, Smith BD. 2006. The molecular genetics of crop domestication. Cell 127:1309?1321. 11 Heun M, Schaferpregl D, Klawan D, Casta- gna R, Accerbi M, Borghi B, Salamini F. 1997. Site of einkorn wheat domestication identified by DNA fingerprinting. Science 278:1312?1314. 12 Matsuoka Y, Vigouroux Y, Goodman M, Sanchez J, Buckler E, Deobley J. 2002. A single domestication for maize shown by multilocus microsatellite genotyping. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:6080?6084. 13 Olsen K, Schaal B. 2006. DNA sequence data and inferences on cassava?s origin of domestica- tion. In: Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, editors. Documenting domestication: new genetic and archaeological paradigms. Berke- ley: University of California Press. p 123?134. 14 Londo JP, Chiang Y, Hung H, Chiang T, Schaal B. 2006. Phylogeography of Asian wild rice, Oryza rufipogon, reveals multiple inde- pendent domestications of cultivated rice, Oryza sativa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:9578?9583. 15 Smith BD. 2001. Documenting plant domes- tication: the consilience of biological and archaeological approaches. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:1324?1326. 16 Emshwiller E. 2006. Genetic data and plant domestication. In: Zeder MA, Bradley D., Emshwiller E., Smith B, editors. Documenting domestication, new genetic and archaeological paradigms. Berkeley: University of California Press. p 99?122. 17 Smith BD. 2006. Documenting domesticated plants in the archaeological record. In: Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, edi- tors. Documenting domestication, new genetic and archaeological paradigms. Berkeley: Uni- versity of California Press. p 15?25. 18 Doebley J. 2006. Unfallen grains: how an- cient farmers turned weeds into crops. Science 312:1318?1319. 19 Jaenicke-Despre?s V, Buckler E, Smith BD, Gilbert M, Cooper A, Doebley J, Pa?a?bo S. 2003. Early allelic selection in maize as revealed by ancient DNA. Science 302:1206?1208. 20 Jaenicke-Despre?s V, Smith BD. 2006. An- cient DNA and the integration of archaeological and genetic approaches to the study of maize domestication. In: Staller J, Tykot R, Benz B, editors. Histories of maize. Amsterdam: Elsev- ier. p 83?97. 21 Zeder MA. 2006. A critical assessment of markers of initial domestication in goats (Capra hircus). In: Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, editors. Documenting domestica- tion: new genetic and archaeological para- digms. Berkeley: University of California Press. p 181?209. 22 Childe VG. 1951. Man makes himself. New York: The New American Library of World Lit- erature. 23 Smith B. 2001. The transition to food pro- duction. In: Price TD, Feinman G, editors. Archaeology at the millennium: a sourcebook. New York: Klewer Academic/Plenum. p 199? 229. 24 Richerson P, Boyd R, Bettinger R. 2001. Was agriculture impossible during the Pleisto- cene but mandatory during the Holocene? Am Antiquity 66:387?411. 25 Burroughs W. 2005. Climate change in pre- history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 26 Hayden B. 1992. A new overview of domesti- cation. In: Gebauer AB, Price TD, editors. Tran- sition to agriculture in prehistory. Madison: Prehistory Press. p 11?19. 27 Kennett D, Winterhalder B, editors. 2006. Behavioral ecology and the transition to agri- culture. Berkeley: University of California Press. 28 Spencer CS. 1997. Evolutionary approaches in archaeology. J Archaeol Res 5:209?264. 29 Price TD, Gebauer A, editors. 1995. Last hunters, first farmers: new perspectives on the transition to agriculture. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press. 30 Wetterstrom W. 1993. Foraging and farming in Egypt: the transition from hunting and gath- ering to horticulture in the Nile Valley. In: Shaw T, Sinclair P, Andah B, Okpoko A, edi- tors. The archaeology of Africa. New York: Routledge. p 165?226. 31 Flannery K, editor. 1986. Guila? Naquitz. Or- lando: Academic Press. 32 Piperno D. 2006. A behavioral ecological approach to the origins of plant cultivation and domestication in the seasonal tropical forests of the New World. In: Kennett D, Winterhalder B, editors. Behavioral ecology and the transition to agriculture. Berkeley: University of California Press. p 137?166. 33 Smith BD. 2006. Rivers of change, 3rd ed. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press. 34 Kuijt I, Goring-Morris N. 2002. Foraging, farming, and social complexity in the pre-pot- tery Neolithic of the southern Levant: a review and synthesis. J World Prehist 16:361?440. 35 Bar-Yosef O, Belfer-Cohen A. 2002. Facing environmental crisis: societal and cultural changes at the transition from the Younger Dryas to the Holocene in the Levant. In: Capper RTJ, Bottema S, editors. The dawn of farming in the Near East. Berlin: ex oriente. p 55?66. 36 Rosenberg M. 1990. The mother of inven- tion: evolutionary theory, territoriality, and the origins of agriculture. Am Anthropol 92:399? 415. 37 Rosenberg M. 1998. Cheating at musical chairs: territoriality and sedentism in an evo- lutionary context. Curr Anthropol 39:653? 681. 38 Downs JF. 1966. The significance of environ- mental manipulation in Great Basin cultural development. In: d?Azevedo WL, Davis WA, Fowler DD, Shuttles W, editors. The current status of anthropological research in the Great Basin: 1964. Reno: Social Sciences and Human- ities Publications No 1. The Desert Research Institute. p 39?55. 39 Posey D. 1985. Indigenous management of tropical forest ecosystems: the Kayapo Indians of the Brazilian Amazon. Agroforestry Syst 3:139?158. 40 Yen D. 1989. The domestication of environ- ment. In: Harris D, Hillman G, editors. Forag- ing and farming. London: Unwin Hyman. p 55? 75. 41 Harlan J. 1992. Crops and man. Madison: American Society of Agronomy, Crop Science Society of America. 42 Terrell J, Hart J, Barut S, Cellinese N, Curet A, Denham T, Kusimba C, Latinis K, Oka R, Palka J, Pohl M, Pope K, Williams P, Haines H, Staller J. 2003. Domesticated landscapes: the subsistence ecology of plant and animal domestication. J Archaeol Method Theory 10:323?368. 43 Deur DD, Turner NJ. 2005. Keeping it living, traditions of plant use and cultivation on the Northwest Coast of North America. Seattle: University of Washington Press. 44 Anderson MK. 2005. Tending the wild. Berkeley: University of California Press. 45 Ford RI. 1985. The processes of food pro- duction in prehistoric North America. In: Ford RI, editor. Prehistoric food production in North America. Ann Arbor: Anthropological Papers, 75, Museum of Anthropology, University of Michi- gan. p 1?18. 46 Harris D. 1996. Domesticatory relations of people, plants, and animals. In: Ellen R, Kat- suyosi F, editors. Redefining nature. Oxford: Berg. p 437?463. 47 Lepofsky DI, Lertzman K. 2007. Document- ing ancient plant management in the Northwest of North America. Can J Botany. In press. 48 Fish SK. 2000. Hohokam impacts on So- noran Desert environment. In: Lentz DL, editor. Imperfect balance: landscape transformations in the Precolumbian Americas. New York: Co- lumbia University Press. p 251?280. 49 Fish SK, Fish PR. 1992. Prehistoric land- scapes of the Sonoran Desert Hohokam. Popul Environ 13:269?283. 50 Fish SK, Fish PR, Madsen J. 1992. Evidence for large-scale agave cultivation in the Mariana community. In: Fish SK, Fish PR, Madsen J, editors. The Mariana community in the Hoho- kam world. Tucson: Anthropological Papers No. 56. University of Arizona Press. p 73?117. 51 Smith BD. 2001. Low level food production. J Archaeol Res 9:1?43. 52 Bettinger RL. 1989. The archaeology of Pinyon House, Two Eagles, and Crater Mid- dens: three residential sites in Owens Valley, Eastern California. New York: Anthropological Papers No. 67, American Museum of Natural History. 53 Munson P. 1986. Hickory silviculture: a sub- sistence revolution in the prehistory of eastern North America. Paper presented at the confer- ence on emergent horticultural economies of the eastern woodlands. Carbondale, Illinois. 54 Delcourt PA, Delcourt HR. 2002. Prehistoric Native Americans and ecological change. Cam- bridge: Cambridge University Press. 55 Piperno D. 2006. Prehistoric human occupa- tion and impacts on neotropical forest land- scapes during the Late Pleistocene and Early/ Middle Holocene. In: Bush MB, Flenley JR, edi- tors. Tropical rainforest responses to climatic change. Springer. p 193?217. 56 Mellars P. 2006. Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:9381?9386. 57 Erickson D, Smith BD, Clarke A, Sandweiss D, Tuross N. 2005. An Asian origin for a 10,000-year-old domesticated plant in the Amer- icas. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:18315?18320. 58 Bleed P. 2006. Living in the human niche. Evol Anthropol 15:8?10. 59 Moray D. 1994. The early evolution of the domestic dog. Am Sci 82:336?347. 60 Zeder MA. 2006. Archaeological approaches to documenting animal domestication. In: Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, editors. Documenting domestication: new genetic and archaeological paradigms. Berkeley: University of California Press. p 171?181. 198 Smith ISSUES 61 Kislev ME, Hartmann A, Bar-Yosef O. 2006. Early domesticated fig in the Jordan Valley. Sci- ence 312:1372?1374. 62 Lev-Yadun S, Ne?eman G, Abbo S, Flaish- man MA. 2006. Comment on ??Early domesti- cated fig in the Jordan Valley.?? Science 314:1683. 63 Kislev M, Hartmann A, Bar-Yosef O. 2006. Response to comment on ??Early domesticated fig in the Jordan Valley.?? Science 314:1683. 64 Smith BD. 1997. The initial domestication of Cucurbita pepo in the Americas 10,000 years ago. Science 276:932?934. 65 Smith BD. 2006. Seed size increase as a marker of domestication in squash (Cucurbita pepo). In: Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, editors. Documenting domestication, new genetic and archaeological paradigms. Berke- ley: University of California Press. p 25?32. 66 Piperno D, Stothert K. 2003. Phytolith evi- dence for early Holocene Cucurbita domestica- tion in southwest Ecuador. Science 299:1054? 1057. 67 Piperno D. 2006. Identifying manioc (Man- ihot esculenta) and other crops in pre-Colum- bian tropical America through starch grain analysis. In: Zeder MA, Bradley DG, Emshwiller E, Smith BD, editors. Documenting domestica- tion, new genetic and archaeological para- digms. Berkeley: University of California Press. p 46?67. 68 Weiss E, Kislev M, Hartmann A. 2006. Au- tonomous cultivation before domestication. Sci- ence 312:1608?1610. 69 Weiss E, Wetterstrom W, Nadel D, Bar- Yosef O. 2004. The broad spectrum revolution revisited: evidence from plant remains. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:9551?9555. 70 Piperno D, Moreno JE, Iriarte J, Holst I, Lachniet M, Jones JG, Ranere AJ, Castanzo R. 2007. Vegetation and climate history of the Iguala Valley, Central Balsas Watershed, Mex- ico, during the Late Pleistocene and Holocene periods. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. In press. 71 Binford LR. 1968. Post-Pleistocene adapta- tions. In: Binford S, Binford LR, editors. New perspectives in archaeology. Chicago: Aldine de Gruyter. p 313?341. VC 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Forthcoming Articles ? Unity in Diversity: Lessons from Macaque Societies Bernard Thierry ? Towards a Map of Capuchin Monkeys? Tool Use Eduardo B. Ottoni and Patricia Izar ? Advances in Optically Stimulated Luminescence (OSL) Dating of Individual Grains of Quartz from Archaeological Deposits Zenobia Jacobs and Richard G. Roberts ? Speculations About the Selective Basis for Modern Human Craniofacial Form Daniel E. Lieberman ? The Morphological Distinctiveness of Homo sapiens and Its Recognition in the Fossil Record: Clarifying the Problem Ian Tattersall and Jeffrey H. Schwartz ? Statistical and Biological Definitions of ??Anatomically Modern?? Humans: Suggestions for a Unified Approach to Modern Morphology Osbjorn M. Pearson ? New Developments in the Genetic Evidence for Modern Human Origins Timothy D. Weaver and Charles C. Roseman ? The Origin of Modern Anatomy: Speciation or Intraspecific Evolution? Gu?nter Bra?uer ? Homo in the Middle Pleistocene: Hypodigms, Variation, and Species Recognition G. Philip Rightmire ISSUES Behavioral Context of Plant and Animal Domestication 199