Biol. Rev. (2000) 75. pp. 649-669 Printed in the United Kingdom ? Cambridge Philosophical Society 549 The functional significance of silk decorations of orb-web spiders: a critical review of the empirical evidence M. E. HERBERSTEIN1*, C. L. CRAIG23, J. A. CQDDJNGTON4 and M. A. ELGAR1 1 Department of Apology, University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia 2 Museum of Comparative CT) 1^ ?i ?? CM CT> oc 01 ? 3 CO ? CM -o-> a. 3 o ? c/ t/. ? X* TJ u ? 2 c c o ft 33 XI if) CO r-~ co CTl CD 5i <*3 o>< CT) U _ ^ - PQ p c c CM ___ O -~J o a o .*. CM 01 a c a -ts u t: ?** ~ C ? 9 ? C -? -? c3 c3 ^ -? o * be u _Q & hp nj v co co , - CT> ai ai -? ? CTl en a> ? U ?' ?? ? d Q ?' ??' ^^ rt o o o rt a, x rt C u C o X > u ? a a G- > > u a; id C c C >? o o o aj iu a_> aj 2 o -O o V ??c o J3 o V c ID v ? O - Q. '3 .'S >; rt d V u O, O - - ^ ? tJ ? o cj o 3 u y d c a. O O x -a s oj >^ "oj "aj C o .a ^ ? = u ooooo-cjiiOi; o ^ o 2 -222 ? ^ u o ~v a "2 "33 id- - -Q ^ w ^;-ys OJ , rr a; a; t> aj OJ Jp U u u u ?? U Ui O ^ (-. -^ o -o 2 u ? o -a x) JH - u 2 ? 2 2 O aj -O y3 S ^ d ^ XJ XI TD X3 ^d^^ddc.-dddd X! XI XI d d d 5^ K^5 5^ a ^ pe D. ?a o* I & I. ?S- o ^ > > b ^ ^ ?> -a "^ S 'C a H -2 po S s s c *o _? a ta te !< ? & C -; ^ ? a M, _^- _ g g a _v: 5 ^ e K G ^> g ^. cr t/ t./ i^ ^. 5 ? a a 9 q a ? ^ J C *?* ? ?^ J o^ o* V> cr r^1 a B ^0 sy) -^ -^ i: a ^^>^<0'0^TN^^ ^^^^-^^i^co^o ? 3 u 3 ?& ?- c Silk decorations of orb-web spiders 653 O CO 't 00 i? i^? i~? r^ tJ! o ra CT> -^-~ ""!. 00 en c c c o o o (A n tn ?a .a S IS IS IS o o o Cn Cn 00 ^ ^ Q* - t^ ^ c^ O c 5 p ?2 3 5 \J tA t/i O C c 1 o o ^ ^ ^_( ^-/ U W o c4 ,-, =o en a 2 o -? co c c o S Pi cy5 ~ 08 ?' bp ~ > 9 c X 2 -a u rt Z oU LO 05 Ol CTi CT> ?? " c c o g bo a. c ? ^5 o -c o .-~. cy ^ ?? "35 ft- ?O * ? r- o D O c/3 IX -3 H ro oo ?. M M O be B 2 CM ? CT) 00 ?. en ?. ? ? u, 2 V h o c Rl M Jj s UU t4 a ! o 1 QJ > > u i- c V a) c a Rj > - B 1 > 1 CO B 'C t) X c .2 u u xi 0 y ft 0 a. o "0 2 i u IU ~ V o c_ C_ g 2 u ?JO o (J3 '5 u V to u u u p X ? ?* v u ? E u u y Q 0 o V c a 0 a e -S ^" 2 "to fe b S C3 ? ?u C3 d a. CO 3 -a a =: | Li 5" d a a CO 3 co a CO B cu 5 CJ ^3 Ar gi op e pi ct a, A. A. re in wa rd ti, A. A. a rg en ta ta , A. V ar io us Ar gi op e v a rio us e a b En a CO a _2 Ne og ea sp . V ar io us Ar gi op e A. a u ra n tia Ar gi op e fl or ida , A A. a pp en sa 0 > o > 0 > 1 ?i 0 Ed > 0 > ?JO a ~ o ?a q 5 ?X) c -5 2 "5 0 ?a ?-"5 - &S ?a 3 b o - - cH X S "3 O o Z Pi "3 - > bo u -- 7. CO CO tu 7: T he rm c W eb pr 654 M. E. Herberstein and o Table 2. Patterns of web decorations in orb-web spiders (Na = not available) Taxon Decoration type Habitat Sour Uloboridae Conifaber parvus Lubinella morobensis Octonoba octonarius O. sybotides O. varians Philoponella herediae P. republicana P. tingens P. undulata Uloborus americanus U. bispiralis U. centiculatus U. conus U. crucifasciens U. diversus U. filifaciens U. glomosus U. plumipes U. scutifaciens Zosis geniculatus Araneidae Arachnura sp. Argiope aemula A. aetherea A. appensa A. argentala A. aurantia A. aurocinta A. bruennichi A, catenulata A. clarki A. doboensis A. flavipalpis A. fiorida A. keyserlingi A. lobata A. minuta A. picta A. pulchella A. radon A. reinwardti A. savignyi A. trifasciata Caerostris sp. Cyclosa bifida Linear Spiral, linear Discoid, spiral, linear Discoid, linear, spiral Spiral Discoid Linear Linear Linear Linear Linear Linear Linear Cruciate Spiral, linear Linear, cruciate Discoid, linear Spiral, linear Linear, irregular mat Discoid, linear Debris Cruciate Cruciate Cruciate Cruciate Cruciate Cruciate Linear Tri-radiate Linear Discoid, cruciate Discoid, cruciate Cruciate Discoid, cruciate Linear Cruciate Cruciate Cruciate Linear Cruciate Discoid, cruciate Discoid, linear Linear Debris Forest Na Na Na Na Forest floor Monsoon forest understory Na Tropical forest understorv Na Tropical understory Cactus hedges Tropical forest understory Against tree trunks Na Palm leaves Urban (campus) Na Against tree trunks Na Na Na Urban (campus) Edges, and forest gaps Grass clearing Prairie Parkland Grassland 'Among trees' Between trunks of palms Tree trunks Lower shrub Na Shrub Na Na Na Mango trees Over river Lubin etal. (1982) Lubin (1986) Peaslee & Peck (1983) Watanabe (1999 a) Yaginuma (I960) Opell (1987) Opell (1979) Opell (1979) Lubin (1986) Cornstock (1912) Lubin etal. (1982) Hingston (1927) Lubin etal. (1982) Hingston (1927) Eberhard (1973) * Hingston (1927) Cushing & Opell (1989) Marples (1969); Peters (1993) Hingston (1927) Lubin (1986) N. Scharfl" (pers. comm.) Robinson & Robinson (1974) Elgar el al. (1996) Kerr (1993) Craig & Bernard (1990) Tso (1998 a) Robinson & Robinson (1980) Malt (1993) Hingston (1927) Hingston (1927) Na Na Prairie Na Na Robinson & Lubin (1979) Edmunds (1986) Eisner & Nowicki (1983) Herberstein el al. (2000) Robinson & Robinson (1974) Yaginuma (1960) Robinson & Robinson (1974) Marson (1947a) Levi (1983); Robinson & Robinsoi (1980) Robinson & Robinson (1974) Nentwig & Heimer (1987) Tso (1998 a) J. A. Coddington (pers. obs.) Bristowe (1941) Silk decorations of orb-web spiders 655 Table 2. (cont. Taxon C. caroli C. conica C. centrifasciens C. cylindrifasciens C. insulana C. micula C. turbinata Cyrtophora bifurcata C. citricola C. hirta C. moluccensis Gasteracantha brevispina G. cancriformis G. curvispina G. germinata G. minax Gea eff Isoxya cicatricosa Micrathena sexspinqsa Neoscona domiciliorum N. arabesca Neogea egregia Parawixia tuberculata Singa sp. Witica crassicauda W. crassispina ZUla sp. ] ZHIa sp. 2 Tetragnathidae Nephila clavipes M. edulis ?N. maculata N. plumipes N. tetragnathoides Nephilengys cruentata Decoration type Discoid, spiral, debris Linear, debris Debris Silk cylinder Discoid, spiral, debris Spiral debris Linear, Debris Debris Debris Debris Debris Linear, tufts Tufts Tufts Linear, tufts Cruciate Linear Linear - Linear Linear Linear Debris Na Linear Linear, tufts Linear Linear Linear Linear, debris Linear (juveniles) Linear, debris Linear, debris Debris Habitat Source Na Shrubs On trees Mangroves Rubber trees Na Na Na Na Na Na Na Na Under bushes Na Shrub Tall grass Grass Na Na Na Na Na Na Na Na Bushes, forest edge Na Forest edges Forest Various Mangroves Coconut plantations Urban Levi (1977) Bristowe (1941) Hingston (1927) Hingston (1927) Marson (1947*) Bristowe (1941) Rovner (1976) Levi (1977) Kullmann (1958) M. A. Elgar (pers. obs.) M. A. Elgar (pers. obs.) Hingston (1927) Marples (1969) Edmunds & Edmunds (1986) Simon (1895) Mascord (1970) Levi (1983) Edmunds & Edmunds (1986) Nentwig & Heimer (1987) McCook (1889) McCook (1889) Levi (1983) Yaginuma (1960) Wiehle (1929) Levi (1986) Nentwig & Heimer (1987) Robinson & Robinson (1980) Robinson & Robinson (1980) Robinson & Robinson (19734) M. A. Elgar (pers. obs.) Robinson & Robinson (1973a) M. A. Elgar (pers. obs.) M. A. Elgar (pers. obs.) Edmunds & Edmunds (1986) : . Ulobonds also show ontogenetic change in web- building behaviour. Immature Uloboms conus built inear decorations, while adults rarely built any (Lubinid ai, 1982). Zosis gemculatus adults con- structed disks more than linear decorations, while in ~?srdecorat,ons were m?re **?? (2) Population variation The proportions of individuals that decorate their webs can vary greatly between different populations For imtancc A. Jlavipalpis in West Africa (Edmunds, y?b and U. diversus in Arizona, USA (Eberhard, 197J) almost always spun web decorations By contrast only 25?/o of individuals of A. appensa observed on Guam constructed web decorations (Hauber, 1998), while on neighbouring Pacific islands the frequency of web decorations in A. appensa varied between 3.6?/0 and 76% (Kerr, 1993) Similarly, the proportion of A. argentata that never 656 M. E. Herberstein and oth Table 3. Summary of the types of web decorations and their possible location on the web (see Fig. 1 for schematic representations) Type Location on web Silk Discoid Hub Spiral Hub Linear Hub Cruciate Hub Tufts Orb. frame Debris Prey remains Orb, barrier web Egg sacks Orb, frame, barrier web Example Juvenile Argiope argentala Lubinella morobensis Argiope trifasciata A. argentala Various Gasleracantha spp. Mephila plumipes Cyrtophora citricola Nentwig & Heimer (1987) Lubin (1986) Horton (1980) Craig & Bernard (1990) Edmunds (1986) Austin & Anderson (1978) Edmunds & Edmunds (19{ discoid Fig. 1. Schematic representation of various web decoration types in orb-web spiders. decorated their webs ranged fronT65 % in Panama (Robinson & Robinson, 1970) to between 58% and 78% on Santa Cruz Island, Galapagos (Lubin, 1975). These data may either reflect facultative changes in the decorating behaviour according to local environmental conditions, or indicate a genetic basis for web-decorating behaviour (Edmunds, 1986). (3) Individual variation Individuals of the same species display different decorating behaviours that may be influenced by changes in local physical factors. For example, A. aetherea spun more web decorations in dim light than in bright light (Elgar, Allan & Evans, 1996). By contrast, U. diversus constructed more circular ai linear decorations following nights of bright i lumination compared with nights of low light levt (Eberhard, 1973). Food availability also affec decorating behaviour. For example, A. aurantia,. trifasciata (Blackledge, 1998 A) and A. keyserlh (Herberstein, Craig & Elgar, 2000) construct mo and larger web decorations when they are mail tained on a high-energy diet than when maintainf on a low-energy diet. Octonoba sybotides was moi likely to construct spiral decorations when foe deprived while food-satiated individuals tended I form linear decorations (Watanabe, 19996). Fu thermore, when A. keyserlingi was fed the san amount of prey, the number and size of decoratioi was greater when prey encounter rates were ui Silk decorations of orb-web spiders 657 predictable than when they were constant (Herberstein et al., 2000). The effects of feeding regimes were less pronounced for A. trifasciata foraging in the field, which suggests that other environmental factors, in addition to nutrition, influence the decorating behaviour (Tso, 1999). Finally, experimental data suggest that selection may favour unpredictable decorating behaviour. For example, the stingless bee, Trigona fluviventris, learned to avoid the webs of A. argentata that were decorated identically over successive days. When the orientation of decorations was varied, however, 7". ^ fluviventris were more likely to be intercepted and hence captured in the web (Craig, i994 Augusta 1 Macracantha ? isoxya Austracantha Togocantha Aetrocantha ? Gasteracantha a Scoloderus a Acanthapeira Hyposinga m Singa a Zygiella Kaira =a Metapaira Dolophones Anepsion Colphapeira o Nuctenea ?i Cyclosa ? Araneilla ea Eriophora a Varri/coss a IWanff0f3 Neoscona Q Larinia Aculepeira i Araneus i Cercidia l Pronous i Metazygia ? Alpaida i Enacrosoma i Bertrana jEustala Wixia Acacesia CO J3 bp 3 cr 0 c d o c o L "3 tf s a- o bo | -a o G M IS o U a ? Q o -a U Silk decorations of orb-web spiders G59 (A) (B) Fig. Sec Decoration patterns unordered I I absent r?x^1 linear spiral discoid ?? cruciate ?? tufts liiitil polymorphic 1 | equivocal 3. Cladogram tracing decoration patterns on equally weighted tree for Uloboridae (A) and Tetragnathidae (B). text for further details. functions. Moreover, it is likely that the same type of decoration differs in function between species. behaviour. We discuss each in turn, together with a critical analysis of'the available evidence. VI. FUNCTIONAL EXPLANATIONS The evolutionary significance of decorating behav- iour has attracted considerable debate, and there are several general explanations for the function of this (1) Mechanical function Simon (1895) was the first to ascribe a mechanical function to the cruciate decorations constructed by A. argentata, suggesting that the silk ribbon 'strengthens the position occupied by the spider". -.- 660 ? M. E. Herberstein and 0| (A) o -c a o t O ? o e o i a to 5 s 5 D o 0) 3 ? 2 tt < o -c o I ?S -C S c CO I ,^ c ? QJ o -* Uj s 13 ? ? D Decoration patterns unordered I I absent l:::::::::l linear W7\ spiral HU1] discoid ?? cruciate BB debris ?? tufts ETS5I polymorphic 1=1 equivocal Fig. 4. Cladogram tracing decoration patterns on an equally weighted tree for Araneidae. The tree is split into parts (A, B) for clarity. See text for further details. Accordingly, the silk bands may enable the spider to adjust the mechanical state of the completed web (Robinson & Robinson, 1970). Therefore, the variability in web decorations may reflect differences in the amount of mechanical adjustment require strengthen the web (Robinson & Robinson, 19 For example, Robinson & Robinson (1# attributed a mechanical strengthening functid Silk decorations of orb-web spiders 661 the zigzag bands on the moulting platforms of Nephila clavipes. This idea has been tested indirectly by comparing the frequency with which decorations are added to webs at sites characterised by different wind con- ditions. The proposed role of decorations is to stabilise webs in windy sites, where they are likely to require 'mechanical support'. Hence, more decorations should be added to webs in windy sites than to webs spun under calmer conditions. These observations provide little supporting evidence; there was no difference in the frequency of decorations.spun by A. argentata in habitats exposed to strong winds and those in sheltered habitats - (Lubin, 1975;Nentwig&Rogg, 1988). Nevertheless, Neet (1990) reports an increase in the frequency of circular decorations in Cyclosa insulana on windy days. However, these observations were conducted on three separate days, two of which were classified as 'calm' and data on wind speed (or any other environmental variables including rate of prey capture) are not provided (Neet, 1990). Wind speed manipulated in the laboratory had no effect on the decorating behaviour of A. argentata (Nentwig & Rogg, 1988). A mechanical function of web decorations seems unlikely, although the idea has not been tested extensively. For example, it is not known whether ~ decorated webs are more stable than undecorated webs, nor is it clear how web decorations could stabilise or strengthen the YVjeb. Indeed, other types of web decorations, particularly debris or tuft: decorations (Fig; 1), seem an unlikely strengthening tool because the decorating silk is.generally laid as an unstressed mass and often only attached to a single thread (Eberhard, 1973,. 1990). Perhaps the strongest supporting evidence will come from com- parative studies that compare the mechanical stab- ility of webs constructed by decorating and non- decorating species. (2) Anti-predator function Web decorations may protect spiders from predatory attacks. There are thought to be several different protective mechanisms that depend, in part, on the various types of decorations. Pellets of prey remains or egg sacks may act as a decoy, confusing the predator who attacks the pellet rather than the spider (Hingston, 1927). Spiral or discoid decorations may conceal and hide the spider from predators (Eberhard, 1973; Ewer, 1972) and cru- ciate or linear bands may change the apparent shape of the spider, making it less obvious to predators (Edmunds, 1986). Cruciate decorations may also increase the apparent size of the spider by appearing as extensions of pairs of legs, and thus protect it from gape-limited predators (Schoener & Spiller, 1992). Web decorations, including the silk tufts used by Gasteracantha spp., may protect spiders indirectly by advertising the presenceof the web, which may be a negative stimulus to some avian predators (Horton, 1980). The anti-predator function for web decorations has been examined in a number of ways. First, variation in web decorations has-been interpreted to "- reflect the relative abundance of predators. For example, the frequency of decorating A. argentata on different islands in the Galapagos is associated with the number of potential predators (Lubin, 1975). Spiders on Daphne Island, which lacks potential spider predators, only rarely spin web decorations, whereas spiders spun decorated webs more fre- quently on Santa Cruz Island, which maintains a higher diversity of predators such as birds and lizards. However, there are no reports of direct predatory attacks by any of the suggested predators (Lubin, 1975). It is also possible that there may be ?other differences between the two islands that affect decorating behaviour. U A survey of A. argentata in the Bahamas showed that cruciate decorations were mostly spun by medium-sized spiders" hut not by small or large .spiders (Schoener & Spiller, 1992). A. argentata may ~ use cruciate decorations to increase their apparent size to predatory lizards (Anolis spp.), whose gape size places an upper limit on the size of their prey. Consequently, medium-sized spiders:"benefit most -from spinning decorations but small spiders could not increase their apparent size beyond the gape size of the lizard artd large spiders are already large enough (Schoener & Spiller, 1992). While this explanation is certainly possible, the mortality rate due to predatory attack of differently sized A. argentata was not measured nor was the response of lizards towards decoration size tested experimentally (Schoener & Spiller, 1992). Finally, the variation in web decorating may reflect a trade-off between foraging efficiency and predator avoidance (Blackledge, 1998 6). Thus, well- fed spiders might invest more energy into predator avoidance by spinning more decorations compared with food-deprived animals. Laboratory experi- ments revealed that food-deprived A. trifasciata constructed fewer web decorations than well-fed spiders (Blackledge, 19986). 662 M. E. Hcrberstein and other* A more direct approach attempts to demonstrate experimentally that the risk of predation is reduced by decorations. Captive blue jays (Cyanocilla cristata) were allowed to choose individuals of A. argentata and A. trifasciata without a web, spiders in undecorated webs and spiders in decorated webs. The birds attacked spiders without a web most frequently, and spiders in undecorated webs more frequently than those in decorated webs (Horton, 1980). These data are consistent with the anti-predator function, but the decorations may not have acted to camouflage or conceal the spider, but rather the birds may have learned to associate web decorations with a negative stimulus - a sticky web (Horton, 1980). The anti-predatory function of decorations is widely cited (Table 1) despite the paucity of experimental evidence. The absence of decorating behaviour in nocturnal species is consistent with the anti-predator explanation because few visual predators are likely to be active at night. However, records of the prey of diurnal sphecid predators reveal no consistent patterns. For example, the predominant genera captured by Sceliphron laetum in Madang, PNG were Argiope, Gasteracantha and Neoscona (Elgar & Jebb, 1999); only the latter is not known to decorate webs. By contrast, S. madra- spatanam conspicillatum rarely captured Argiope spp., but preyed more frequently on species ofAraneus and Neoscona '(Adato-Barrion & Barrion, 1981). Of "course, it is difficult to interpret these data without knowledge of the abundance and accessibility of the different genera. However, the possibility that predators may also learn to associate web decorations with prey location is a strong argument against an anti-predator function (Robinson & Robinson, 1970). In a held experiment, crickets attached to screens were offered as prey to wild birds in the field. The crickets were placed in the centre of a 'model stabilimentum' consisting of a white zig-zag stitching arranged in a cruciate pattern. Eventually, the birds showed a preference for crickets with models over crickets without models (Robinson & Robinson, 1970). (3) Improving foraging success There are essentially two mechanisms by which web decorations may increase the foraging success of web-building spiders. First, they may attract pol- linating insects by reflecting UV light in patterns similar to UV markers on flowers. Second, UV patches created by web decorations may indicate gaps in vegetation, which elicits flight behaviour in many insects (Craig & Bernard, 1990). A foraging function has been tested extensively using spider that construct cruciate, linear and discoi< decorations (eg. Craig, 1991; Craig & Bernard 1990; Hauber, 1998; Tso, 1996, 1998 a, * Watanabe, 1999a; sec Table 1), and there are tw< lines of supporting evidence. First, the presence ant the number and size of decorative bands wen related to the presence of prey and to prei interception rates of the web. Second, more direc studies using choice experiments and field experi ments showed that insects approach UV-reflectin; web decorations more frequently-than webs withou UV-reflecting decorations. The orientation and presence of decorative sill on webs spun by naturally foraging A. argentata wet manipulated, and the prey-capture rates wet estimated by recording the web-damage patten caused by prey interceptions (Craig & Bernan 1990). Decorated webs intercepted significant! more insects than undecorated webs and the pre ence of the spider itself, which is characterised by reflecting abdomen, further enhanced pre"V-captui rates. Moreover, a within-web comparison showt that the vertical web half containing the decorati\ band also intercepted more prey than the ui decorated web half (Craig & Bernard, 1990). A further field study measured the prey-inte ' ception rates at A. argentata webs under thn conditions: interception rates of solitary spider web interception rates of webs within 3 m of at least or other web, and interception rates of webs with boi the spider and the -decorations removed (Crai 1991). Webs of A. argentata foraging within 3 in each-other intercepted more insects than solitar foragmg individuals.- Furthermore, within the clusters, decorated webs captured more prey tha undecorated webs (Craig, 1991). However, wht the spiders and the decorations were remove solitary webs intercepted insects at similar rates webs in a cluster (Craig, 1991). The presence of web decorations was also relati to prey interception in A. trifasciata (Tso, 199( Decorated and undecorated webs did not differ size, but decorated webs intercepted more flyii insects than undecorated webs. However, the w half containing a decorative band did not captt more flying insects than the web half without decorative band (Tso, 1996). Similarly, A. keyserlv captured more prey on webs with decoration ban than on undecorated webs (Herberstein, 200 Observations of A. appensa on Guam revealed differences in the prey-interception rates Silk decorations of orb-web spiders 663 ? decorated and undecorated webs, and no increase in prey interception in the decorated quadrants of the same webs (Hauber, 1998). This result was con- founded by differences in web size: decorated webs were significantly smaller than undecorated webs. After controlling for this size difference, decorated webs intercepted more prey per unit web area than undecorated webs (Hauber, 1998). The relationship between web decorations and prey capture has been examined in field studies of Cyclosa conica, which constructs linear decorations in its horizontal web (Tso, 19986) and Octonoba sybolides, which utilises linear and spiral decorations (Watanabe, 1999a). Decorated webs of G. conica intercepted 150% more insects than undecorated we-bSj even though they were 19% smaller than undecorated webs (Tso, 19986). Similarly, when the webs of 0. sybotides were adorned with either spiral or linear decorations, they intercepted more insects compared with undecorated webs. The size of decorated and undecorated webs did not differ (Watanabe, 1999a), although the mesh height of webs decorated with spirals was smaller than that of undecorated webs. However, this may not affect the conclusions because webs with a smaller mesh must also be more visible to insect prey than webs with a larger mesh (Craig, 1986; Watanabe, 1999a). Three manipulative experiments provide more conclusive evidence in support of a foraging function. In choice experiments, Drosophila spp. flies were confronted-with identical webs spun by Uhbprus glomosus (Craig & Bernard, 1990) and -O. sybotides (Watanabe, 1999 a). The web in one-arm of a Y- maze was illuminated with white light containing a UV component, while the web in the-other arm-was illuminated with white light without a UV com- ponent. Flies approached and were captured in webs .containing -U V-reflecting decorations more frequently than in webs that did not reflect UV. Furthermore, isolated bands of decorations built by A. aurantia were transferred onto an artificial web consisting of a synthetic adhesive mesh and exposed in the field (Tso, 1998a). In a control group, a similar-sized area of non-decorative silk was intro- duced into identical artificial webs. The artificial webs were then installed adjacent to already existing A. aurantia web sites. Artificial webs containing decoration bands captured significantly more flying insects than those in the control group, while web site or date had no effect on prey capture (Tso, 1998 a). Blackledge and Wenzel (2000) recently published the results of an experimental study that tested the response of honeybees to silks spun by ancestral [Plerinochilus sp.) and derived spiders (A. aurantia) against a grass background. On the basis of their experiments they argue that they were unable to train bees to associate an award with UV-reflecting silk concluding that decorations spun by orb- spinning spiders are cryptic to bees which would not be able to discriminate UV-reflecting silk from background vegetation. Blackledge and Wenzel (2000) hypothesise that the reflectance spectra of the decorative silks.are flat but, regrettably, do not provide data to test this assertion. This is important because the only spectral; data available show a 30% variation- in the reflectance spectrum of decoration silks spun by A. argentata (-100% reflexive-at 370 nm varying to approximately-70% reflectance at 640 nm; Craig & Bernard, 1990). It is unknown whether there were any differences-.in the spectral properties of the ancestral and derived silks or the silks and their background. Further, most vegetation [with the ex- ception of densely hairy or glaucous leaves and foliage (Frolich, 1976)].absorbs, light in the UV to blue (330-420 nm) region of the spectrum and re- flects light in the green region of the spectrum and above ( > 550 nm). As a result, the contrast between the web decoration and a green background is high (see Figs 2 a andja in Craig & Bernard, 1990). Blackledge & Wenzel (2000) have shown that honey- bees had difficulty associating thtdecorative silks with the sugar reward, but learning is a different be- haviour from .perception. In fact, honeybees have more difficulty learning to associate a sugar water reward witfrUV-reflecting objects than objects that reflect any other wavelength (Menzel & Erber, 1978). Finally, the-achromatic reflectance (bright-, ness) of the silks and the background was not- measured. Thus, we cannotdetermine if the observed choices were made in response to brightness or colour of the silks and their background. The experiments cited above show that understanding insect responses to colour, pattern and brightness is extremely com- plex and will require tightly controlled experiments that address one variable at a time. In a field experiment (Blackledge & Wenzel, 1999), A. aurantia in decorated webs captured fewer prey than spiders in webs that had the decorations removed, suggesting that web decorations do not function to increase foraging success, but actually reduce it. This study is puzzling, since Tso (19986) used the same species (see above) and reported the opposite effect: artificial traps containing decorations intercepted more prey than control traps M E. Herberstein and others 664 ? nossible explanations, blackleage ) usea . b size between the Thus random dinerences ui ?i.u > SSWSS'SSS and a? spiders ran Variation in the attack Sedg * ? 1999) may h?? difcen. Seville prey P?P^r(T?Pa.r998ir and aurmlia in Michigan, USA (iso, IW? I iherefore serve a different function. Several other studies that investigate the source of varSSn in decorating hehavtour have ako proved A!n that are consistent with a foraging function. I or datathataec ed (helr ?5?| behaviour according to light condition, t mm light they adorned their webs with more and a nrg de?crtia,eydeeora,ions than in bng^hgh preLmahly increasing *? ???*; TJV reflection: if decorations increase prey inter - SSS53S5wS* ??*? if web deeorTti"* are par. of a foraging strategy, spide s should vary the decorating behaviour ? ???? variation in the rate of prey encounter (?"?*"". Til 2000) A. hymhni ?m maintained on the P rtlnt to note that a mechanical or anti-predator ZcZZoZpredict these changes in decorating IXlur according to ambient light condition or *??? response of ^-?? d^ti^^ decorations (Blackledge, 1998ft). In A. kyserhnp CaSjSn - o/.s 2000) and .4. trtffao* d-* 1999), food-deprived spiders also increased thesize of th r webs. These patterns may reflect a trade-off be ween the costs and benefits of decorating be- haviour In the field, decorated webs suffer trom 'reacr web damage due to prey interception fHauber 1998). Consequently, hungry spiders may ieTceweb damage of their larger webs by spinning fewer decorations (Herberstein et al., 2000). I ^may also be that web decorations target ^-??? prev. In this case, starving spiders could be predl??V to spin large, undecorated webs that mtercept prey nom penally while well-fed spiders build sma ler decorated webs captunng finly those insects that ^Sir^^Ssence of.decorating be havLm^amonglocturnal spiders, is ?*?jg with a foraeing function. Comparative analyses oe Twe n he prelected by decorating and non- d^radng "specres may provide further insights into a foraging iunction. (4) Decreasing web damage The web decorations constructed by Argiope species or the silk tufts'spun by Gasteracantha^species are Conspicuous structures, "and may advertise the presence of webs to birds or other larger animals that PT otherwise- encounter and destroy the web Z ner & Nowicki, 1983). This idea was tested xper mentally by placing paper replicates of cru- ciateTe orations made by A. fionda on the vacant ^D^ul^ie.ofngturnaJv^W widens During the day, webs with a paper strip Offered a ^r^C^^n***^ .domed webs (Eisner & Nowicki, 1983). It B Scuhto aSSesl these data because the^sou? rf web destruction is not identified and there are a dumber of potentially biasing effects (see also Craig ft Semard 1990). For example, there was no control L dXnce! 1 the ability of webs to absorb insect kineu! energy, the primary factor causing web hreikdown and which varies greatly between webs ofTfferent species (Craig, 1987) Henoe,som webs are destroyed when they mtercept large oi fas flvL insects, while others are unaffected^ It is un ikel hat he kinetic properties of the webs spun by sixdifferent araneid species are identical, and Aerefore the differences in damage may be caused bv varyL rates of insect interception. Similarly the reflec iv "properties of the pieces of paper used in he rtudy we- not measured and it is likely that Irey offered from those of silk decorations, which Silk decorations of orb-web spiders may have very different effects on insect prey or even birds. However, a recent field experiment using the webs and decorations of A. aurantia showed that webs with decorations suffered less damage than webs that had all decorations removed (Blackledge & Wenzel, 1999), while the presence of the spider in the web did not affect the rate of web damage. Empty frames were initially set up in a triangular arrangement on a mown lawn and birds were lured to the middle of the arena with a dish of bird seed, After a period of acclimatisation, two of the three empty frames were replaced by a web containing decorations and a web where all decorations had been removed. Whi e Bhckledge & Wenzel (1999) did not provide information on .the. source of web destruction, they observed several birds actively avoid webs that contained decorations. In a different approach, Kerr (1993) related the frequency of web-decorating behaviour with the presence of birds on different Pacific islands, Only 16 4?/ of A. appensa spun decorated webs on Guam, where the introduction of the brown tree snake (Boiga irregulans) has eliminated all of the native birds over the past 30 years (but see Hauber, 1998). By contrast, between 41.9 % and 56.9 % of A. appensa on the neighbouring islands of Rota, Tmian and Saipan* where the bird fauna remains intact, decorated their webs (Kerr, 1993). Perhaps the extinction of birds on these islands reduced or eliminated selection for web-decorating behaviour. This evolutionary scenario suggests that the cost of decorating is very high if this behaviour is to be lost in such a short time interval. The main argument against an advertisement function is that many spiders that build decorations locate their webs in sheltered positions (see Table 2; Eberhard, 1990) such as tree buttresses (e.g. Lubinella spp.), tall grass (e.g. Argiope spp.) shrubs and undefstory (e.g. Gasteracantha spp.) where birds are unlikely to fly through and damage the web (Eberhard, 1990). Experimental designs that expose these webs outside their natural shelter (e.g. on a mown lawn in Blackledge & Wenzel, 1999) may not provide biologically relevant information. (5) Thermoregulation A few studies suggest a thermoregulatory function for disc-shaped decorations. Humphreys (1992) found that the discs constructed by juvenile Neogea spp. provided shade to spiders foraging in sites characterised by high temperatures. When tempera- tures exceeded 40 ?C, the spider moved under the disc where it was protected. The disc cut the transmission of light by 60%, reducing the body temperature of the spider by 1.8 ?C (Humphreys, 1992). It is likely that this shuttling behaviour evolved secondarily, with the animal taking ad- vantage of an existing structure. However, web decorations are found in a variety of different light conditions, and more frequently under dim light (Elgar et al., 1996). Furthermore, linear or cruciate decorations do not shade the spiders' body and are thus of limited thermoregulatory benefit. VII. NON-FUNCTIONAL EXPLANATIONS Nentwig & Heimer '(1987) argue that the high degree of unexplained variation in decorating be- haviour, together with a body of contradictory evidence, indicates that web decorations are unlikely to have an evolutionary function. Instead, web- decorating behaviour may simply arise as a conse- quence of non-specific stress" reactions (Nentwig & Rogg, 1988) or silk regulation (Peters, 1993). Under laboratory conditions, A. argentata varied their decorating "behaviour in response to extreme temperatures, the presence -of males, moulting and age, factors that were thought to raise levels of stress (Nentwig & Rogg, 1988). However, these patterns were not replicated under field conditions, where variation in temperature, humidity, illumination and wind velocity failed to explain differences in web-decorating behaviour (Nentwig & Rogg, 1988). Peters (1993) suggests that web decorations are a by- product of silk regulation. Not all of the silk from the aciniform (Araneidae) and piriform (Uloboridae) glands, is used in wrapping prey, and thus the superfluous silk is transferred into the decorations (Peters, 1993). This idea has not been- tested empirically. There are several objections to the non-functional explanation of silk decorations. First, the energetic cost of silk production is high (Peakall & Witt, 1976; Opell, 1998). Therefore, it is unlikely that natural selection will maintain web-decorating behaviour unless it provides some benefit. Furthermore, it would be surprising if a non-functional trait evolved independently several times, as appears to be the case. The stress-response interpretation is counter- intuitive; spiders that experience stress should not deplete their nutritional resources further by pro- ducing more costly silk. In fact, spiders spin ? ',-' ' 666 decorations under extremely benign laboratory conditions that minimise or eliminate 'stress' (e.g. Herberstein et al., 2000). The inter-specific variation in web-decorating behaviour provides little support for either the stress-response or silk-regulation explanations. For example, such explanations should apply equally to spiders that build webs during the day or night, but web decorating is not found in nocturnal species. Additionally, it is not clear why spiders belonging to the genus Argiope suffer higher stress levels or need to regulate silk more than spiders of, say, the genus Araneus that do not decorate their webs '(Table 1). Finally, web decorations are typically constructed immediately after the orb web is completed. Even if the spiders have produced superfluous silk, it seems unlikely that they would discard it at the beginning of a foraging bout when an unknown quantity of silk is needed to wrap prey. While an excess of silk probably does not induce decorating behaviour, it is likely that the availability of silk in the glands will affect the frequency and size of web decorations. The amount of wrapping silk expended by A. aetherea- has been manipulated experimentally by offering some spiders more prey, whiclf was consequently wrapped but not ingested (I.M/Tso, unpublished data). These spiders spun fewer or smaller decorations in subsequent webs "than.spiders whose silk reserves were not depleted. At the end of the foraging period, the spider's reserve oraciniform silk was ..never completely exhausted, and spiders were always able to release more silk to wrap-prey (I. M. Tso, unpublished data). VIII. CONCLUSION The function of web decorations remains an un- resolved issue. At least seven different functional explanations have been proposed, but only a few functions have been tested directly. The foraging function has been tested most extensively on linear, cruciate and circular decorations and is supported by several experiments and correlative field studies (e.g. Craig & Bernard, 1990; Craig, 1991; Hauber, 1998- Tso, 1996, 1998 a, b; Watanabe, 1999 a). The anti-predatory function (Horton, 1980) and the web advertisement function (Blackledge & Wenzel, 1999) have been investigated for linear, cruciate and circular decorations built by Argiope spp. By contrast, the other functions and other types of decorations have not been tested as extensively. Assigning a specific function to web decorations is made difficult for a number of reasons. First, the M. E. Herberstein and others selective pressures responsible for the evolution of web decorations may differ from those identified by contemporary studies as maintaining the behaviour. Second, the convergent nature of decorations and their patterns implies that different types of decorations serve different functions in different species. For example, the relationship between prey capture and web decorations is confirmed for the linear and cruciate types, particularly in Argiope spp. and may also apply to disc-shaped decorations spun by some Uloboridae (Craig & Bernard, 1990; Watanabe, 1999 a). However, it is not clear whether debris types have similar functions. In fact, it is not clear whether the eggsacs placed Jn_the web of Cyclosa, Arachnura and Gyrtophora species should be considered as decorations equivalent to the linear, circular or cruciate patterns. Keeping eggsacs in the web may allow the spider to protect the eggs from predators, parasites and parasitoids (see Elgar et al, 1983). While these egg sacs-may provide additional concealment, the selective 'route' is.fundamentally different to those species that use acinifo/m silk bands. Similarly, the prey items placed in bands in the webs of Nephila species may function primarily as food storage, with concealment being a selectively neutral consequence. ., Finally, the different types of decorations con- structed by juvenile and adult spiders also impede an .attempt to assign particular functions. While on- togenetic changes suggest distinct mechanisms or perhaps different functions for the different decorations types, these are still poorly understood. The vast majority of studies have been concerned with adult rather than juvenile spiders. Several steps may help to resolve the ongoing controversy regarding the function _qf web decorations. First, it may be helpful to identify four phylogenetic clusters of web decorations:' uloborine' uloborids, 'argiopine' and 'gasteracanthine' araneids and 'nephiline' tetragnathids. Within these clusters, similar web decorations may have similar functions as a result of common ancestry. Extra- polations from one phylogenetic locus to another are unlikely to be relevant in resolving this debate. Second, the different decorating patterns may describe different phenomena that have undergone different selective routes. Therefore, the functions of the various types of decorations may or may not be convergent. For example, debris structures should not be termed 'decorations' or 'stabilimenta', but should be treated as separate behavioural phenom- ena. Two main clusters remain within the 'true' silk types of decorations. The 'bright white' silk bands Silk decorations of orb-web spiders spun frequently by species in the 'argiopine' and 'uloborine' cluster, which may be convergent in form and function, (although evidence is not very strong at this stage), and the more dull decorations that are spun less frequently by species in the 'gasteracanthine' and 'nepheline' clusters. Finally, inter-specific comparative studies that combine field observations, experimental studies and the life histories of decorating and non-decorating spiders should give the clearest insight into the selective factors that have influenced the evolution ol decorating behaviour among spiders. IX. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Astrid Heil.ng, I-Min Tso, Mark Hauber Bill Piel and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and criticisms. Alexander Kerr generously provided a list of decorating species. Financial support was provided by the Austrian Science Foundation (J150Q- BIO) to M.E.H., the Australian Research Council (A19930103) l0 M.A.E., the Bunting Institute and the American Association of University Women (AAUW) to C 1 C and NSF (NSF DEB-9712353; DEB-97-07744), the Smithsonian-Neotropical Lowlands and Scholarly Studies Programs, to J. A. C. X. REFERENCES ADAI'0-BARR.ON, A. M. & BARRION, A. T. (1981), Spider prey of Sceliphrm madraspatanum conspicillatus (Hymcnoptera: Sphecidae}-. Kalikasan 10, 122-125. AUSTIN, A. D. & ANDERSON, D. T. (1978). Reproduction and development of the spider Nephila edulis (Koch) (Araneidae: . Araneae). Ausl. J. '~52- 667 COMSTOCK, J. H. (1912). The spider book. Doubleday, Page & Company, New York. COMSTOCK,.]. H. (1940). The spider book. Doubleday, Doran & Company, Inc., New York. CRAIG C L (1986). Orb visibility, the influence of insect Hignt J behaviour and visual physiology on the evolution of web designs within the Araneoidea. Amm. Behav. 34, 54-68. CRAIG C L. (1987). The significance of spider size to the diversification of spider-web architectures and spider re- productive modes. Am. Nat. 129, 47-68. CRAIG, C. L. (1989). Alternative foraging modes of orb web weaving spiders. Biotropica 21, 257-264. CRAIG C L (1991). Physical constraints on group foraging and social evolution: observations on web-spinning spiders. Fund. Ecol. 5, 649-654. CRAIG C L (1994a). Limits to learning: effects ol predator pattern and colour on perception and avoidance-learning by prey. Anim. Behav. 47, 1087-1099. CRAIG C L (1994A). Predator foraging behavior in responses ' perception and learning by its prey: interactions between orb- spinning spiders and stingless bees. Behav. Ecol. Soaobwl. 35, 45-52 CRAIG, C. L. (1997). Evolution of arthropod silks. Annu. Rev. -.Entomoi. 42, 231-267. CRAIG C L. & BERNARD, G. D. (1990). Insect attraction to ultraviolet-reflecting spider webs and web decorations. Eulogy 71.616-623. I A /,QQi\ - CRAIG, C. L., BERNARD, G. D.& CODDINGTON, J. A; (U )? Evolutionary shifts in the spectral properties of spider silks. Evolution 48, 287-296. CUSHING, P. E. & OPELL, B. D. (1989). Disturbance behaviours in the spider Uloborus glomosus (Araneae, Ulobndae). Can. J. Zool. 68, 1090-1097. ^ EBERHARD, W. G. (1973). Stabilimenta on the webs of Uloborus diversus (Araneae: Uloboridae) and other spiders. J. Zool. Lond. 17LJ67-384. EBERHARD, W. G. (1990). Function and phylogeny of spider webs". Armtr.Rev.Ec'ol. Syst. 21, 341-37E : EDMUNDS J (1986). The stabilimenta of Argiope flampalpis and Areiope 'tnfasciala in West Africa, with a discussion of the .. function of stabilimenta. In Proceedings of the 9th International Confess of Arachnology, Panama 1983 (eds-W. G, Eberhard, Y.D.Lubin and B. C. Robinson.), pp. 61-72. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington. EDMUNDS, J. & EDMUNDS, M. (1986). The defensive mechanisms of orb weavers (Araneae: Araneidae) in Ghana, West Africa. In Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Arachnology, Panama 1983 (eds W. G. Eberhard, Y. D, Lubin and B.C. Robinson.), pp. 73 89. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington. EISNER T & NOWICKI, S. (1983). Spider web protection through visual advertisement: role of the stabilimentum. Science 219. 185-187. ELGAR M. A., ALLAN, R. A. & EVANS, T. A. (1996). Foraging strategies in orb-spinning spiders: ambient light and silk decorations in Argiope aetherea Walckenaer (Araneae: Araneoidea). Aust. J. Ecol. 21, 464-467. ELGAR M A. &JEBB, M. (1999). Nest provisioning in the mud- dauber wasp Sceliphron laetum (F. Smith): body mass and taxa specific prey selection. Behaviour 136, 147-159. ELGAR M A., POPE, B. & WILLIAMSON, I. (1983). Observations on the spatial distribution and natural history of Cyrtophora 668 M. E. Herberstein and others hirta (L. Koch) (Araneae: Araneidae) in Queensland, Australia. Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 6, 83-87. EWER, R. F. (1972). The devices in the web of the African spider Argiope flavipalpis. J. Mat. Hist. 6, 159-167. FOELIX, R. F. (1992). Biologic der Spinnen. Gcorg Thieme Verlag Stuttgart. FROLICH, M. W. (1976). Appearance of vegetation in ultraviolet light: absorbing flowers, reflecting backgrounds Science 194 839-841. GRISWOLD, C. E, CODDINGTON J. A., HORMIGA, G., & SCHARFF, N. (1998). Phylogeny of the orb-web building spiders (Araneae, Orbiculariae: Deinopoidea, Araneoidea) Zool 7 Linn. Soc. 123, 1-99. GRISWOLD, C. E., CODDINGTON, J. A., PLATNICK, N I & FORSTER, R. R. (1999). Towards a phylogeny of entelegyne spiders (Araneae, Entelegynae). J. Arachnol. 27 53-63 HAUBER, M.E. (1998). Web decorations and alternative foraging tactics of the spider Argiope appensa. Elhol. Ecol Evol 10, 47-54. HERBERSTEIN, M. E. (2000). Foraging behaviour in orb-web spiders (Araneidae): do web decorations increase prey capture success in Argiope keyserlingi Karsch 1978? Aust. 7 Zool 48 217-223. ' ' HERBERSTEIN, M. E., CRAIG, C. L. & ELGAR, M. E. (2000). Foraging strategies and feeding regimes: web and decoration investment in Argiope keyserlingi Karsch Mraneae: Araneidae) Evol. Ecol. Res. 2, 69-80. HINGSTON, R.-W. G. (1927). Protectivedevices in spider's snares, with a description of seven new species of orb-weaving spiders Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 28, 259-293. HORMIGA, G., EBERHARD, W. G. & CODDINGTON, J. A. (1995). Web construction behavior in Australian Phonognatha and the phylogeny of nephiline ami tetragnathid spiders (Araneae, Tetragnathidae). Aust. J. Zool. 43, 313-343. HORTON, C. C. (1980). A defensive function for the stabilimenta of two orb weaving spiders (Araneae: Araneidae). Psyche 87 13-20. -_ ----- '. HUMPHREYS, W. F. (1992). .Stabilimenta as parasols: shade construction by Meogea sp. (Araneae: Araneidae, Argiopinae) and its thermal behaviour,.Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 9, 47-52. KERR, A. M. (1993). Low frequency of stabilimenta in orb webs of Argiope appensa (Araneae: Araneidae) from Guam: an indirect effect of an introduced avian predator. Pacific Science 47, 328-337. KOVOOR, J. (1987). Comparative structure and histochemistry of silk-producing organs in arachnids. In Ecophysiology of spiders (ed. W. Nentwig), pp. 160-186. Springer Verlag, Berlin. KULLMANN, E. (1958). Beobachtung des Netzbaues und Beitrage zur Biologie von Cyrtophora citricola Forskal (Araneae, Araneidae). Zool. Jb. Syst. 86, 181-216. LEVI, H. W. (1977). The orb-weaver genera Cyclosa, Melazygia and Euslala north of Mexico (Araneae: Araneidae). Bull. Mus Comp. Zool. 148, 61-127. LEVI, H. W. (1983). The orb-weaver genera Argiope, Gea, and Meogea from the western Pacific region (Araneae: Araneidae, Argiopinae). Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool. 150, 247-338. LEVI, H. W. (1986). The orb-weaver genus Witica (Araneae- Araneidae). Psyche 93, 35-46. LUBIN, Y. D. (1975). Stabilimenta and barrier webs in the orb webs of Argiope argentata (Araneae, Araneidae) on Daphne and Santa Cruz Islands, Galapados. J. Arachnol. 2, 119-126. LUBIN V. D. (1986). Web building and prey capture in the Ulobondae. In Spiders- Webs, behavior, and evolution (ed W A Shear), pp. 132-171. Stanford University Press, Stanford, C*A. LUBIN, Y. D., OPELL, B. D., EBERHARD, W. G. &LEVI, H. VV. (1982). Orb plus cone-webs in Uloboridae (Araneae),' with a description of a new genus and four new species. Psyche 89, MADDISON, VV. P. & MADDISON. D. R. (1992). MacClade- Interactive analysis of phylogeny and character evolution Version 3.0. Sunderland, IVlassachusetts: Sinauer Associates. MALT, S. (1993). Trophicai relations of selected web-building spiders lAraneae) in xerophil grasslands. Boll. Ace. Gioenia Sci Nat. 26, 253-261. MARPLES, B.J. (1969). Observations on decorated webs. Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 1, 13-18. MARSON, j.E. (1947a). Some observations on the ecological variation and development of the cruciate zigzag camouflage device of Argiope pulchella (Thor.). Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 117 219-227. MARSON, J. E. (1947A). Some observations on the variations in the camouflage devices used by Cyclosa insulana (Costa), an Asiatic spider, in its web. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 117, 598-605 MASCORD, R. (1970). Australian spiders in colour. Charles E Tuttle Co., Vermont. MCCOOK, H. C. (1889). American spiders and their spinningwork I Academy of Natural Sciences of Philadelphia, "Philadelphia MENZEL, R. & ERBER, J. (1978). Learning and memory in bees Scient. Am. 239, 102-109. NEET, C. R. (1990). Function and structural variability of the stabilimenta of Cyclosa insulana (Costa) (Araneae, Araneidae) Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 8, 161-164. NENTWIG, W. & HEIMER, S. (1987). Ecological aspects 6tspider webs. In Ecophysiology of spiders (ed. W. Nentwig), pp. 211-225 Springer Verlag, Berlin. NENTWIG, VV. & Rooo, H. (1988). The cross stabilimentum of Argiope argentata (Araneae: Araneidae) - nonfunctional or a nonspecific stress reaction? Zool. Ana. 221 246-266 - OPELL, B. D. (1979). Revision of the genera and' tropical American species in the spider family Uloboridae. Bull Mus Comp. Zool. 148, 445-549. OPELL, B. D. (1987). The new species Philoponella herediae'&nd its modified orb-web (Araneae," Uloboridae). J. Arachnol. 15, 59-63. OPELL, B. D. (1998). The material cost and stickiness of capture threads and the evolution of orb-weaving spiders Biol T. Linn. Soc. 62, 443-458. ' PEAKALL, D. B. & WITT, P. N. (1976). The energy budget of an orb web-building spider. Comp. Biochem. Physiol 54A 187-190 PEASLEE, J. E. & PECK, W. B. (1983). The biology of Octonoba octonanus (Muma) (Araneae, Uloboridae). 7. Arachnol 11 51-67. *' PETERS, H. M. (1993). Uber das Problem der Stabilimente in Spinnennetzen. Zool. Jb. Physiol. 97, 245-264. RABAUD, E. (1932). Le role du stabilimentum des toiles d'Argiope fasciee. C. R. Acad. Sci. 194, 655-657. ROBINSON, B.C. & ROBINSON, M. H. (1974). The biology of some Argiope species from New Guinea: predatory behaviour and stabilimentum construction (Araneae: Araneidae) Zool J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 54, 145-159. ROBINSON, B. & ROBINSON, M. H. (1978). Developmental studies of Argiope argentata (Fabricius) and Argiope aemula (Walckenaer). Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 42, 31^0. ^ Silk decorations of orb-web spiders ROBINSON, M.H. & LUB.N, Y. (1979). Specialists and generalises: the ecology and behavior of some web-bu.ldmg spiders from Papua New Guinea. I. Herennia omatissima, Arg>op? ocyaloides and4r?fomm??to?*w(Arancae: Arancidae). Par./?. 21,97-132. ROB.NSON, M. H. & ROBINSON, B. (1970). The stabilimentum of the orb web spider, Argiope argenlata: an improbable defence against predators. Can. Enl. 102, 641-655. ROB.NSON, M.H. & ROBINSON, B. (1973a). Ecology and behavior of the giant wood spider tophila maculala (* abacus) in New Guinea. Smith. Contr. fool. 149, 1-76. ROBINSON, M. H. & ROB.NSON, B. (19734). The stabilimenta of .Nephila clampes and the origins of stabilimcntum-bu.lding in araneids. Psyche 80, 277-288. - ROBINSON, M.H. ScROB.NSON, B. (1980). Comparative slud.es of the courtship and mating behav.or of trop.cal arane.d spiders. Pac. Ins. Mono. 36, 1-218. ROVNER J S (1976). Detritus stabilimenta on the webs ot Cydosa turbinata (Araneae, Araneidae). J. Arachnol. 4,215-216. SCHARFF N. & CODDINOTON, J.A. (1997). A phylogent.c analysis of the orb-weaving spider farmly Araneidae (Arachnida, Araneae). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 120, 355-424. SCHOENER, T.W. & SP.LLER, D. A. (1992). Stabilimenta characteristics of the spider Argiope argenlata on small islands: support of the predator-defense hypothesis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 31, 309-318. SIMON, E. (1895). Historie naturelle des Araignees. Roset, Pans. 669 THIRUNAVUKARASU, P., NICOLSON, M. & ELGAR, M. A. (1996). Leaf selection by the leaf-curling spider Phonognatha graejei (Keyserling) (Araneoidea: Araneae). Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 10, 187-189. TOLBERT.W. W. (1975). Predator avoidance behaviors and web defensive structures in the orb weavers Argiope aurantia and Argiope trifasciala (Araneae: Araneidae). Psyche 92, 29-52. Tso I. M. (1996). Stabilimentum of the garden spider Argiope trifasciala:* possible prey attractant. Anim. Behav. 52, 183-191. Tso I. M. (1998a). Isolated spider web stabilimentum attracts insects. Behaviour 135, 311-319. Tso I M. (1998A). Stabilimentum-decorated webs spun by Cydosa conica (Araneae, Araneidae) trapped more insects than underrated webs. J. Arachnol 26, 101-105. Tso I M (1999). Behavioral response of Argiope trifasciala to recent foraging gain: a manipulative study. Am. Midi. Nat. 141,238-246. WATANABE, T. (1999a). Prey-attraction as a possible function ot the silk decoration of the uloborid spider Oclonoba sybohdes. Behav. Ecol. 5, 607-611. WATANABE, T. (1999A). The influence of energetic state on the form of stabilimentum built by Oclonoba sybohdes (Araneae: Uloboridae). Ethology 105, 719-725. WIEHLE H (1929). Weitere Beitrage zur Biologie der Araneen, insbesondere zur Kenntnis des Radnetzbaues. Z Morph. Okol. Tiere 15, 262-306. YAGINUMA, T. (1960). Spiders of Japan in colour. Hoikusae, Osaka.